Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 09 September 2020
Sec. Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic Current MEG Research in Psychiatry View all 11 articles

Frontoparietal Network Connectivity During an N-Back Task in Adults With Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • 1Department of Diagnostic Imaging, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 2Neurosciences & Mental Health Program, SickKids Research Institute, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 4Neuropsychology and Functional Neuroimaging Research Group, Center for Research in Cognition & Neurosciences and ULB Neuroscience Institute, Université Libre de Bruxelles (ULB), Brussels, Belgium
  • 5Laboratoire de Cartographie Fonctionnelle du Cerveau, Hôpital Erasme, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
  • 6Bloorview Research Institute, Holland Bloorview Kids Rehabilitation Hospital, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 7Department of Neurology, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 8Department of Paediatrics, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 9Department of Medical Imaging, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada

Background: Short-term and working memory (STM and WM) deficits have been demonstrated in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and may emerge through atypical functional activity and connectivity of the frontoparietal network, which exerts top-down control necessary for successful STM and WM processes. Little is known regarding the spectral properties of the frontoparietal network during STM or WM processes in ASD, although certain neural frequencies have been linked to specific neural mechanisms.

Methods: We analysed magnetoencephalographic data from 39 control adults (26 males; 27.15 ± 5.91 years old) and 40 adults with ASD (26 males; 27.17 ± 6.27 years old) during a 1-back condition (STM) of an n-back task, and from a subset of this sample during a 2-back condition (WM). We performed seed-based connectivity analyses using regions of the frontoparietal network. Interregional synchrony in theta, alpha, and beta bands was assessed with the phase difference derivative and compared between groups during periods of maintenance and recognition.

Results: During maintenance of newly presented vs. repeated stimuli, the two groups did not differ significantly in theta, alpha, or beta phase synchrony for either condition. Adults with ASD showed alpha-band synchrony in a network containing the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, bilateral inferior parietal lobules (IPL), and precuneus in both 1- and 2-back tasks, whereas controls demonstrated alpha-band synchrony in a sparser set of regions, including the left insula and IPL, in only the 1-back task. During recognition of repeated vs. newly presented stimuli, adults with ASD exhibited decreased theta-band connectivity compared to controls in a network with hubs in the right inferior frontal gyrus and left IPL in the 1-back condition. Whilst there were no group differences in connectivity in the 2-back condition, adults with ASD showed no frontoparietal network recruitment during recognition, whilst controls activated networks in the theta and beta bands.

Conclusions: Our findings suggest that since adults with ASD performed well on the n-back task, their appropriate, but effortful recruitment of alpha-band mechanisms in the frontoparietal network to maintain items in STM and WM may compensate for atypical modulation of this network in the theta band to recognise previously presented items in STM.

Introduction

Adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) demonstrate difficulties with a variety of executive functions (14), one of which is working memory (WM), which refers to the ability to hold and manipulate information in mind (5, 6). WM is related to short-term memory (STM), which involves the mental storage of information for a short period of time (7, 8), and which is also impaired in adults with ASD (912). Given their link with each other, and their influence on cognitive capabilities, such as intelligence and academic achievement (1318), understanding the nuances and the extent of STM and WM impairments in ASD is a crucial first step in improving cognitive outcomes in this population. The current literature points to a more severe deficit in ASD in visual, especially visuospatial, aspects of STM and WM, rather than in verbal STM and WM (10, 1923), though people with ASD exhibit impairments in both modalities (11, 24). Neuroimaging work has additionally shown that individuals with ASD exhibit atypical neural activity and connectivity during visual STM tasks (2527) and in both visual and verbal WM tasks (2832).

Several functional neuroimaging studies of STM and WM have demonstrated activation of a frontoparietal network (3339) consisting mainly of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), which includes the superior and middle frontal gyri (SFG and MFG), and of the inferior parietal lobule (IPL). The frontoparietal network is thought to exercise cognitive control to adapt to rapidly changing goals and demands (4044) that certainly occur in STM and WM tasks. The IPL has been primarily associated with maintenance (4551), which entails the temporary storage of information in STM or WM, though the dlPFC has also been implicated (46, 48, 5256). The dlPFC is additionally involved in recognition of previously presented or repeated stimuli (5760), a function which encompasses access to or selection of relevant stimulus representations in STM or WM, and it also plays a role in updating or manipulation of information in WM. Much of the STM and WM literature in ASD has utilised paradigms tapping both maintenance and recognition processes; during such tasks, individuals with ASD show differential activation of this frontoparietal network across development, exhibiting increased activity in the dlPFC during childhood (27, 61), but the opposite in adulthood (26, 31, 62). Moreover, they show poor modulation of these frontoparietal regions with increasing cognitive load (30, 63, 64).

More recent work has examined not only the activation of regions in the frontoparietal network, but also how they communicate or synchronise with each other and with other brain areas. These connections are thought to be fundamental for exerting top-down control on other areas and networks for successful task performance (41, 42, 6570). In the ASD population, studies generally demonstrate that areas in the frontoparietal network are less coupled with each other and with other regions of the brain (26, 29, 31). This reduced functional connectivity suggests impairments in integrating information amongst brain areas during maintenance and recognition, which may contribute to ASD symptomatology (25, 32). These findings echo the current literature on connectivity in ASD, which posits that individuals with ASD show decreased long-range functional connectivity and altered local connectivity across a range of contexts (7176), suggesting that a deficit in neural communication may account for the cognitive difficulties observed in the ASD population.

Although neural long-range synchrony, especially in the theta and alpha frequency bands, has been linked to STM and WM maintenance and recognition processes (39, 7785), the specific frequency band(s) in which these connectivity differences occur in individuals with ASD have been less explored. To our knowledge, only one study has demonstrated reduced alpha-band connectivity in children with ASD during a WM task, reflecting inefficient processing during recognition of repeated stimuli that was associated with severity of their ASD symptoms (32). Frequency-specific differences in STM- or WM-related connectivity in adults with ASD and their relation to behaviour have yet to be examined, even though these abilities can impact adaptive behaviours in individuals with ASD (3, 86, 87).

Thus, the present study investigated whether adults with ASD demonstrate connectivity differences when engaging STM and WM processes, and if they are frequency-dependent. Adults with and without ASD performed an n-back task, a classic paradigm in which participants view a series of stimuli and are asked to recall whether the current stimulus was also presented n trials earlier (88). We measured and compared connectivity between the control and ASD groups during maintenance and recognition of novel visual stimuli for both 1-back and 2-back versions of the task. The 1-back condition involves mainly STM processes, as an individual is only required to maintain and recognise the stimulus shown in the previous trial in mind. On the other hand, the 2-back condition elicits WM processes, as one must continuously monitor and update information stored in and retrieved from memory. Due to their involvement in STM (38, 39, 83, 84, 8993) and WM mechanisms (7779, 9496) as well as long-range interregional communication (9799), we specifically contrasted phase synchrony in the theta, alpha, and beta frequency bands. We also focused on connections amongst frontoparietal network regions and the rest of the brain, given its known role in STM (3739, 84) and WM (33, 36, 100, 101), and since prior work has demonstrated deficits in this network in ASD (25, 27, 31). As individuals with ASD show deficits in tasks involving STM and WM maintenance (11, 20), decreased brain activity during STM recognition (27), and reduced connectivity during WM recognition (32), we predicted that adults with ASD would demonstrate decreased interregional connectivity during both maintenance and recognition of novel stimuli in STM and WM. We further hypothesised that these differences would appear in the alpha band, in line with our previous findings in children with ASD (32), and given its link with STM and WM processes, especially maintenance (77, 85, 102105).

Materials and Methods

Participants

We recruited 92 adults aged 18–40 years, inclusive, for this study, approved by the Research Ethics Board at the Hospital for Sick Children. Individuals were included if they were not born prematurely, had no MRI or MEG contraindications, and demonstrated an IQ ≥ 70, measured using the full-scale, two-subtest version of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI or WASI-II) (106, 107). Control adults were additionally screened for any developmental, neurological, or psychological disorders. Adults with ASD had a primary diagnosis of ASD by an experienced clinician, which was confirmed by the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-G or ADOS-2) (108, 109). All participants gave informed written consent before taking part in the study.

Participants were excluded if they performed poorly on the task (i.e., ≤50% accuracy on 1-back task or ≥50% false alarm rate), had a low number of correct trials (<40 in each condition) after accounting for artefacts, or poor data quality (e.g., poor head localization in the MEG). We then matched participants in the ASD group with those in the control group on age (within two years) and sex, and subsequently excluded any control participants who could not be matched. As participants tended to perform better on the 1-back than the 2-back version of the task, and as we evaluated the 1- and 2-back data separately, the final samples for these two analyses differed; the sample for the 2-back analysis was a subset of that for the 1-back analysis. For the 1-back task, 39 control adults (26 males; 27.15 ± 5.91 years old) and 40 adults with ASD (26 males; 27.17 ± 6.27 years old) met all inclusion and exclusion criteria. For the 2-back task, 29 control adults (19 males; 26.40 ± 5.79 years old) and 30 adults with ASD (19 males; 26.36 ± 6.26 years old) were included in the analyses. Neither sample differed significantly in age, sex, or IQ, and mean calibrated severity scores on the ADOS for both ASD samples were around 7 (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Demographics for 1-back and 2-back samples.

Experimental Design

Questionnaires

To obtain a standardised measure of WM, we asked participants and their informants (e.g., partner or parent) to complete the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Adult Version (BRIEF-A) (110). This questionnaire assesses difficulties with a variety of executive functions that an individual may experience in everyday life. It provides t scores reflecting the degree of impairment on a particular executive function scale, as well as composite scores. We used t scores on the WM scale of the BRIEF-A, with higher scores denoting more severe deficits in WM. Participants also filled out the Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition (SRS-2) (111). The Total t score was taken to gauge ASD symptom severity.

N-Back MEG Task

Participants performed a visual n-back task with two loads, 1- and 2-back (Figure 1A), to elicit STM and WM processes. This task was used in our previous work examining differences in brain activation and functional connectivity between children with and without ASD (27, 32). Our task protocol was similar; stimuli consisted of novel colourful abstract images presented serially for 200 ms each on a black background. During the interstimulus interval, participants saw a white fixation cross for a random duration between 1,050–1,300 ms. Participants pressed a button if the most recently presented stimulus matched that shown n trials previously.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 The n-back task. (A) Participants performance on two loads of this task — 1-back (top row) and 2-back (bottom row) — were tested in separate blocks. They were instructed to press a button as quickly as possible when they recognised that a stimulus had been repeated one or two trials earlier. Images were presented for 200 ms, and the interstimulus interval varied between 1,050–1,300 ms. (B) A schematic of the time windows used to analyse working memory maintenance and recognition processes, with the 1-back load as an example.

The two loads of the n-back task were run in separate blocks. The l-back load scenario consisted of 285 trials: 190 unique images were presented, and 95 of these were shown again on the subsequent trial. The 2-back load segment of the task contained 330 trials: 220 distinct images were shown, of which 110 were repeated two trials later. Stimuli in the 1- and 2-back loads did not overlap. We refer to trials in which stimuli are presented for the first time as New, and those in which they are shown again as Repeat.

All participants first practised both blocks of the task and were given feedback outside of the MEG scanner to ensure they understood the task requirements. Individuals viewed the task on a rear projection screen 80 cm away from the MEG dewar. Presentation 18.1 software (Neurobehavioral Systems Inc., https://www.neurobs.com/presentation) was used to display the task, as well as record participant responses.

Neuroimaging Data Aquisition

A 151-channel CTF MEG system (Coquitlam, British Columbia, Canada) recorded MEG data at a 600 Hz sampling rate from participants during the task. Adults lay supine with their head in the MEG dewar. Head position was tracked continuously through three fiducial coils on the nasion and left and right pre-auricular points. To reduce noise in the data, an anti-aliasing low-pass filter at 150 Hz and a third order spatial gradient were applied.

A 12-channel head coil in a 3T MRI scanner (MAGNETOM, Siemens AG, Erlangen, Germany) recorded T1-weighted MRI data from participants. A sagittal 3D MPRAGE sequence (TR/TE = 2,300/2.96 ms, FA = 9°, FOV = 192×240×256 mm, voxel size = 1.0 mm isotropic) was utilised. Participants were scanned with radio-opaque markers at the MEG fiducial points to allow for coregistration of functional MEG data with structural MRI data.

Statistical Analysis

Behavioural Data

WM capability in everyday life was compared between adults with and without ASD in the 1- and 2-back samples by examining whether there were group (control vs. ASD) or rater (self vs. informant) effects, as well as an interaction between the two, on the WM scale of the BRIEF-A. Performance on the n-back task was contrasted between groups by assessing the effect of group (control vs. ASD) on accuracy and median response time (RT) for the 1- and 2-back loads independently. Accuracy was assessed using d-prime (d′); hits were correct Repeat trials, and false alarms were incorrect New trials.

We used linear mixed effects models to investigate the effects on BRIEF-A data and t tests for the task performance measures. Analyses were carried out separately for the 1-back and 2-back samples in R 3.5.0 (R Core Team, https://www.r-project.org/). Significant results are reported for p < 0.05.

MEG Data

Preprocessing

MEG data preprocessing and analyses were done using the FieldTrip toolbox (112) in MATLAB 2017b (The MathWorks, www.mathworks.com/products/matlab/). Data were epoched from −1,500–2,000 ms, relative to stimulus onset. Signals were then filtered from 1–150 Hz with a fourth-order Butterworth bandpass filter, with 60 and 120 Hz notch filters. Artefacts from physiological sources (e.g., eyes and heart) were detected and removed with independent component analysis. Trials in which the signal was >2,000 fT or head movement was >5 mm were excluded. Of the remaining trials, only correct New and Repeat trials were used for further analyses.

Forward models based on the single-shell method (113) were created from each participant’s T1-weighted MRI data. Inverse models were constructed using the forward model and constrained to the centroids of the 90 regions of the Automated Anatomic Labeling (AAL) atlas (114). Activity at each centroid was taken to represent that respective AAL region. Time series at these sources were estimated using a linearly constrained minimum variance beamformer (115). The covariance matrix was computed on the MEG signal from −400–800 ms, to which 5% regularisation was applied. The neural activity index was calculated to ensure attenuation of centre-of-head noise biases.

Connectivity

As our task involved strong visual and motor responses, we performed seed-based analyses to focus on connectivity between core regions of the frontoparietal network and the rest of the brain to assess phase synchrony directly related to maintenance and recognition of novel visual stimuli. Therefore, we examined the connections amongst six bilateral regions of interest (ROIs) from the AAL atlas, as well as their links to the other AAL regions (except Heschl’s gyrus and olfactory cortex, as their roles in audition and olfaction are not involved in our task). Our ROIs were chosen based on meta-analyses of n-back studies (33, 35, 36). They consisted of the superior frontal gyri [SFG; (-19, 35, 42) and (20, 31, 44)], medial superior frontal gyri [mSFG; (-6, 49, 31) and (8, 51, 30)], middle frontal gyri [MFG; (-34, 33, 35) and (37, 33, 34)], inferior frontal gyri [IFG; (-47, 30, 14) and (49, 30, 14)], insulae [(-36, 7, 3) and (38, 6, 2)], and inferior parietal lobules [IPL; (-44, -46, 47) and (45, -46, 50)].

Connectivity between each pair of sources was quantified using the phase difference derivative (PDD) (116). Source-estimated data were filtered into our frequency bands of interest with Hamming-windowed FIR bandpass filters at the following passbands: 4–7 Hz (theta), 8–14 Hz (alpha), and 15–30 Hz (beta). The lower and upper stopband frequencies for each filter were at 0.6 and 1.9 times the lower and upper frequency cutoffs of each passband, respectively. To reduce detection of spurious connections due to signal leakage, filtered data were subsequently orthogonalized. The instantaneous phase for each source timeseries in each frequency band was obtained with the Hilbert transform. PDD values were calculated at each time point from −400–800 ms using the method outlined by Tewarie and colleagues (117).

Interregional neural communication during maintenance and recognition of novel visual stimuli was determined by considering phase synchrony values in two time windows: 400–800 ms, after the onset of a New stimulus, and 0–400 ms, following the presentation of a Repeat stimulus, respectively (Figure 1B). These windows were established based on average median RTs in both groups, which ranged from ~425–525 ms, across both loads (see Results section). Regarding recognition, we examined a window from 0–400 ms, post-Repeat stimulus onset and just before the lower end of the average median RT, as it would encompass processing related to successful recognition during correct Repeat trials. We compared phase synchrony in this window to that in a similar window of 0–400 ms, post-New stimulus onset (Repeat > New, 0–400 ms, poststimulus onset), as New trials act as a control condition that involves the first occurrence of the stimulus. To investigate maintenance, we evaluated a window from 400–800 ms, post-New stimulus onset, to prevent capturing any perceptual, encoding, and/or identification functions that may occur during early visual processing. Furthermore, we chose this interval to avoid overlap with our recognition analysis and the baseline window. We contrasted connectivity in this time window in New trials with connectivity in an equivalent window of 400–800 ms, post-Repeat stimulus onset in Repeat trials (New > Repeat, 400–800 ms, poststimulus onset). Mean connectivity at all pairwise connections for each participant and each condition in these comparisons was obtained by standardising PDD values in these windows by the baseline period, –400–0 ms, then averaging the resultant z scores over the entire time window of interest.

Statistical comparisons of within-group connectivity during maintenance and recognition were conducted as described above for both adults with and without ASD. We then tested for statistically significant group differences in each of these scenarios (e.g., Control vs. ASD, New > Repeat for maintenance; Control vs. ASD, Repeat > New for recognition). All within- and between-group comparisons were performed for the 1- and 2-back samples separately. For both types of contrasts, we performed cluster-based permutation testing, as implemented in the Network-Based Statistic toolbox (118), to find networks demonstrating significant differences between conditions and groups. Essentially, the Network-Based Statistic approach begins by performing t tests at each connection and applying a threshold, which we chose to be t = 2.641 (1-back) or t = 2.665 (2-back), which are equivalent to p < 0.005 in their respective samples. The robustness of the largest contiguous network formed from the suprathreshold connections was assessed with permutation testing. A null distribution of maximal network size was obtained by rearranging group labels over 5,000 permutations. This procedure allowed for the calculation of a family-wise error-corrected p value (pFWE) of the observed network. Networks were considered significant at pFWE < 0.05. We used BrainNet Viewer (119) and code provided by Koelewijn and colleagues (120) to visualise these networks.

Brain-Behaviour Relations

We explored whether mean network connectivity in any of our group comparisons was associated with WM abilities as measured by the BRIEF-A, task performance (accuracy and median RT), and with ASD symptom severity. Thus, for any networks that differed significantly between groups, we performed regressions of each the BRIEF-A WM scale scores, d′, median RT, and SRS-2 Total scores on mean PDD values in those networks. We report any significant main effects of mean connectivity and/or its interaction with group for p < 0.05.

Results

Behaviour

On the WM subscale of the BRIEF-A, adults with ASD demonstrated significantly more WM difficulties than controls (Figure 2) in both the 1-back (F(1,43) = 25.56, p < 0.0001, d = 0.65) and 2-back (F(1,38) = 22.18, p < 0.0001, d = 0.64) samples. Adults with ASD, compared to their informants, generally rated themselves higher on the WM scale (1-back: F(1,43) = 19.38, p = 0.0001, d = 0.34; 2-back: F(1,38) = 14.06, p = 0.0006, d = 0.32), indicating a greater number of difficulties with WM.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 T scores on the working memory (WM) scale of the BRIEF-A for the 1-back (left panel) and 2-back (right panel) samples. There were significant main effects of both group and rater on WM scores. ***p < 0.001.

On the n-back task, there were no group differences in accuracy (1-back: t(76.93) = 0.77, p = 0.44, d = 0.17; 2-back: t(55.30) = 0.51, p = 0.61, d = 0.13) in the 1- or 2-back loads (Figure 3A). The two groups also had similar median RTs in the 1-back load (t(76.97) = 1.51, p = 0.13, d = 0.34), but their differences in median RT during the 2-back load approached significance (t(54.71) = 1.93, p = 0.058, d = 0.50), such that adults with ASD had slightly longer median RTs than controls (Figure 3B).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Accuracy (A) and median RT (B) on the two loads of the n-back task: 1-back (left panel) and 2-back (right panel). Adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) showed a trend of having longer median RTs than control adults on the 2-back task (p = 0.058). Analyses of all other task performance measures did not reveal any significant group differences.

Neuroimaging

Maintenance

For the 1-back task, during the maintenance window, both control adults (pFWE = 0.048) and adults with ASD (pFWE = 0.001) recruited aspects of the frontoparietal network selectively in the alpha band. In the control group, the network hub with the most (four) connections was the left IPL, which mainly communicated with other left hemisphere regions, such as the left insula (Figure 4A). The right IPL and IFG were also involved in this network, though they were each only connected to two other regions. In the ASD group, both the right SFG and MFG were the main hubs with five connections each, linking the right dlPFC with the right IPL and with several left posterior regions, including the left IPL and precuneus (Figure 4B). In this network, the right IFG also showed synchrony with the right IPL and precuneus.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4 Networks showing increased connectivity during maintenance of novel visual stimuli (New versus Repeat trials, 400–800 ms, poststimulus onset) during the 1-back and 2-back loads (left) and mean connectivity of these networks between −400–800 ms (right). Node size is scaled by number of connections. Connectivity values are given as z scores. (A) Control adults showed recruitment of a network in the alpha band in the 1-back load (pFWE = 0.048), but not in the 2-back load. (B) Adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) displayed greater connectivity in a network in the alpha band similar between the 1-back (pFWE = 0.001) and 2-back (pFWE = 0.002) samples.

For the 2-back load, adults with ASD continued to show increased alpha-band connectivity during New versus Repeat trials (pFWE = 0.002) in a right dlPFC-left posterior network linking the right SFG hub with the left IPL and precuneus (Figure 4B). Control adults did not exhibit greater engagement of any networks in any frequency band for New compared to Repeat trials for this load. There were no significant group differences in the maintenance interval for either load.

Recognition

Recognition processes in the 1-back load were associated with a trend (pFWE = 0.056) increase in theta-band network connectivity in control adults for Repeat relative to New trials. This network was sparse, consisting mainly of a few connections (two each) amongst the left MFG, right IFG, bilateral IPL, and precuneus (Figure 5). Adults with ASD did not show any differential connectivity for Repeat versus New trials in any frequency band. When comparing the two groups, adults with ASD exhibited significantly decreased theta-band connectivity compared to control adults (pFWE = 0.046) in a network of regions in which the right IFG and left IPL were major hubs (Figure 6).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5 Networks showing increased connectivity during recognition of repeated visual stimuli (Repeat versus New trials, 0–400 ms, poststimulus onset) for the 1-back and 2-back loads in the control group (left) and mean connectivity of these networks between −400–800 ms (right). Node size is scaled by number of connections. Connectivity values are given as z scores. In the 1-back load, control adults recruited a theta-band network, but it was only significant at a trend level (pFWE = 0.056). In the 2-back load, they exhibited greater connectivity in networks in the theta (pFWE = 0.0084) and beta (pFWE = 0.015) bands. Adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) did not show differential connectivity during recognition.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6 Theta-band connectivity in adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) compared to controls during recognition of novel visual stimuli (Repeat versus New trials, 0–400 ms, poststimulus onset) in the 1-back load. (A) Adults with ASD showed significantly reduced (pFWE = 0.046) theta-band connectivity in a network with hubs in the right IFG and left IPL. Node size is scaled by number of connections. (B) Mean connectivity in this network between −400–800 ms in the control (top) and ASD (bottom) groups. Connectivity values are given as z scores. (C) Network connectivity represented as a circle plot. Nodes are colour-coded in the following manner, from top to bottom: medial frontal structures (red), frontal areas (orange), parietal regions (turquoise), temporal areas (blue), medial parietal structures (dark blue), occipital areas (purple).

Within-group analyses for the 2-back load during recognition revealed organisation of networks in the theta (pFWE = 0.0084) and beta (pFWE = 0.015) bands in the control group (Figure 5). The theta-band network primarily involved coordination between the mSFG and temporal regions, for example the right fusiform gyrus. The beta-band network displayed a different topography, with the right IFG having the highest (five) number of connections, followed by the right MFG and left insula (three each). Although adults with ASD in this sample also did not show any differential connectivity for this analysis in any frequency band, no significant group differences were found for the 2-back load.

Brain-Behaviour Relations

Mean connectivity in the theta-band network that differed between groups was not associated with any of our behavioural or clinical measures, nor was its interaction with group (all ps > 0.05).

Discussion

Our study illustrates the complex distinctions in STM and WM processing between adults with and without ASD on both the behavioural and neural level. In our sample, adults with ASD performed equally as well as control adults on our visual n-back task, although there was a tendency for adults with ASD to have longer RTs in the 2-back block, during WM processing. This pattern was also observed by Lever and colleagues (121), who similarly demonstrated that despite being as accurate as controls on an n-back task, adults with ASD took significantly longer to respond. Slower RTs may be indicative of slower processing speed, which has also been reported in ASD ((122127); but see (128, 129)). Whilst it may not affect performance on simple experimental STM or WM tasks, this trend towards longer responses or processing could have more noticeable effects in complex, everyday behaviours. Since deficits in processing speed and WM have been found in individuals with ASD (19, 86, 130132), further work is needed to clarify the link between processing speed and WM abilities in ASD, especially as our ASD group reported WM difficulties on the BRIEF-A. Our neuroimaging analyses examined the underlying neural differences in the frontoparietal network responsible for maintenance and recognition of novel visual stimuli that may contribute to these impairments.

Maintenance

During maintenance of novel visual stimuli, we observed that whilst adults with and without ASD did not differ significantly from each other, they exhibited distinct topologies and sizes of the networks they recruited. During the 1-back task, the alpha-band network recruited by controls was fairly left-lateralized, as the left IPL and left insula showed the most connections. The IPL is an integral part of the frontoparietal network involved in n-back tasks (33, 35, 36), and it may serve to maintain stimulus information in STM and WM (37, 4548, 133136), as well as shift attention to specific items in WM (137, 138). Although the IPL is more commonly associated with spatial STM and WM (e.g., (49, 139145)), there is evidence that it similarly participates in object or image identity STM and WM (33, 36, 37, 50, 146149). The insula has also been associated with object STM capacity (150), but its principal function is in recruiting the frontoparietal network when attentional and executive resources are needed (151153) through its functional connections with the dlPFC (154, 155).

In comparison, adults with ASD demonstrated greater alpha-band interregional synchrony for New versus Repeat trials during the 1- and 2-back loads. The networks recruited in both loads were similar; they had a right frontal-to-left parietal configuration and included the right dlPFC, bilateral IPLs, and precuneus. The dlPFC is a key region in STM and WM processing, employing top-down control to maintain, monitor or update, and manipulate task-relevant information in mind (52, 53, 56, 156159), by focusing attention to target stimulus representations in the IPL (54, 101, 160, 161). The precuneus mediates several higher-order cognitive functions (162164), and given its connections with the IPL, it is likely involved in visuospatial processing (165167) and visual recall (168170) in this task. The particular involvement of the precuneus in the ASD group may reflect greater mental engagement, as our previous work showed that children with ASD activated the precuneus more with heavier cognitive load (27). Whilst we did not detect any significant group differences, the recruitment of additional STM and WM regions — the dlPFC and precuneus — in ASD group compared to controls, as well as of several other brain regions, may reflect effortful maintenance processes in ASD adults.

The particular arrangement of this right frontal, left posterior network not only mirrors previous work finding atypical functional lateralization in ASD during a WM task (31), but also suggests that maintenance of novel visual information is challenging for adults with ASD. In control adults, increasing task load has been associated with bilateral activation of the IPL (171, 172) and dlPFC (34, 173, 174), with a few demonstrating stronger effects in right dlPFC (175177). Greater dlPFC-IPL (frontoparietal) connectivity has also been linked to higher task load (77, 178). Therefore, the strong involvement of the right dlPFC and its connection to the left IPL in the ASD group in both the 1- and 2-back loads indicates that holding visual stimuli in mind is mentally taxing for adults with ASD. In both groups, however, brain regions synchronised selectively in the alpha band, which has been implicated in STM and WM maintenance (77, 84, 85, 91, 103, 105, 179). Thus, our findings demonstrate that adults with ASD utilise appropriate neural mechanisms to successfully maintain novel visual stimuli in STM and WM, but it may be effortful for them.

Recognition

During recognition of repeated visual stimuli, adults with ASD exhibited no differential connectivity between Repeat and New trials for either the 1- or 2-back load. Hence, when contrasted with the control group, they showed significantly decreased theta-band synchrony compared to controls during the 1-back load in a network with hubs in the right IFG and left IPL. The ventrolateral PFC, which includes the IFG, is thought to work with the IPL for active retrieval of information (53). Specifically, the right IFG plays a major role in inhibition (180184) and potentially preventing proactive interference during WM (137, 185187), whereas the IPL, in addition to storing stimulus representations, may be responsible for retrieval (146). Since the selection of relevant information in STM during recognition may not only involve enhancement of target stimulus representations, but also suppression of irrelevant ones, and given evidence that individuals with ASD experience deficits in interference control ((188190); but see (191, 192)), the reduced involvement of the IFG and IPL during recognition may reflect a potential breakdown in regulating interference from other stimuli in STM. This possible deficit in inhibiting task-irrelevant stimuli is further corroborated by the fact that these differences occurred in the theta band, as interregional theta-band connectivity is thought to mediate long-range neural communication, top-down control, and integration of distant regions (99, 193195), especially during retrieval (196).

A similar effect was not found when contrasting the two groups in the 2-back task, which may be attributable to the slightly smaller sample size and therefore less power due to a greater variation in response in this condition. Importantly, the control group showed recruitment of theta-band networks for both loads (and additionally a beta-band network in the 2-back load), whereas the ASD group showed no greater connectivity in either load for Repeat compared to New trials. As the pattern of within-group results was similar across loads, a comparable but subthreshold trend may exist in the 2-back condition.

Taking into account that decreased connectivity has been observed in fMRI in ASD during other n-back tasks (25, 29, 31), our findings in the 1-back condition substantiate reports of long-range underconnectivity in ASD (72, 74, 197199). The specificity of this difference to the theta band is also in line with prior work emphasising the role of theta oscillations in STM (39, 82, 84) and recognition/retrieval (196, 200202). This result is in contrast to our previous work (32), where we found that these differences occurred in the alpha band during recognition of repeated visual stimuli in a WM task. However, there are some key distinctions between these two studies. First, the present work observed that connectivity in the frontoparietal network was only significantly reduced in the ASD relative to control group in the theta band during the 1-back condition, whilst we previously reported decreased alpha-band synchrony in a frontotemporal network in individuals with ASD during the 2-back condition (32). Therefore, it is unclear from our prior work whether frontoparietal network connectivity also differed between individuals with and without ASD in the 2-back condition in either the theta or alpha bands, and whether frontotemporal network connectivity was affected in the current study. Second, our earlier study assessed children with ASD, whereas here we included only adults with ASD; thus, part of this discrepancy may be attributable to maturational processes. As both theta and alpha bands have been associated with STM and WM functions (77, 8285, 95, 196, 203205), it may be that connectivity related to recognition strengthens in the alpha band but weakens in the theta band over development in ASD. Whilst global network efficiency in the theta and alpha bands increases with age (206), these longitudinal changes in frequency-specific, long-range neural connectivity have not been characterised in ASD, neither has the frontoparietal network or any other networks related to STM or WM explicitly. Future work into the developmental trajectory of the spectral component of the frontoparietal network and its relation to STM and WM in ASD will be necessary to clarify these distinct findings in children and adults with ASD.

Another important consideration is that mean connectivity in the theta-band network that differed between groups in the 1-back load was not correlated with our behavioural measures. However, as our analyses probed very specific STM (1-back) and WM (2-back) mechanisms, it may be challenging to relate these fine neural differences to overall task performance and more complex behaviours drawing on WM abilities in everyday life. Therefore, it will be valuable for prospective work to evaluate whether these findings persist in more ecologically valid tasks of STM and WM.

Conclusion

Our neuroimaging study revealed unique aspects of STM and WM maintenance and recognition processes in adults with ASD. We demonstrated that whilst adults with ASD appropriately employ alpha-band oscillatory mechanisms to facilitate maintenance of novel visual stimuli in STM and WM, the distinct topology and extent of the recruited networks suggest that these functions are effortful for individuals with ASD. The strong engagement of maintenance processes may offset the observed atypicalities in theta-band connectivity in the ASD group during recognition of previously presented visual stimuli, at least in scenarios tapping STM. Given the spatial and spectral specificity of our findings, we propose that alpha-band connectivity between the dlPFC and IPL in the frontoparietal network enhances the neural representations of target stimuli during maintenance, thereby countering potentially stronger interference effects that occur during recognition due to reduced theta-band synchrony of the IFG and IPL with other regions of the brain.

We are the only study to date to use MEG to detail these maintenance and recognition processes and their spectral properties in the frontoparietal network in ASD. Thus, additional work is needed to independently validate our findings and interpretations in other investigations of STM and WM functions, especially as the distinct connectivity patterns in the control and ASD groups whilst maintaining novel visual stimuli in STM and WM showed only qualitative differences. It would be important to examine maintenance and recognition in other STM and WM tasks that more clearly separate these processes, as in higher loads of n-back tasks, participants are required to maintain stimuli from previous trials in WM, even after recognising a repeated stimulus. This constraint may explain why we did not observe differential connectivity in the control group during the maintenance window of the 2-back load when comparing phase synchrony during New and Repeat trials, and perhaps why we did not find any significant group differences in networks recruited for maintenance of novel visual stimuli. Furthermore, since our paradigm only included 1- and 2-back loads, we were unable to robustly assess whether adults with and without ASD show discrepancies in network recruitment with increasing cognitive load. As previous work has suggested that individuals with ASD do not show load-dependent modulation of activity in the frontoparietal network (26, 30, 63, 207), the effect of load on network connectivity in ASD is an important consideration. Finally, future work should explore STM- and WM-related connectivity patterns in broader samples of individuals with ASD, as our study included mainly higher-functioning adults with ASD who were able to perform well on our n-back task (though who still reported difficulties with WM in everyday life), and since there may be considerable heterogeneity in network recruitment in the ASD population, which may account for the lack of differential connectivity at the group level during recognition in our ASD sample. Our findings demonstrate, however, atypical frontoparietal network connectivity in adults with ASD when engaging in recognition of repeated visual stimuli, especially in STM, and further research will be essential to uncovering the nuances of these discrepancies.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Hospital for Sick Children Research Ethics Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MT conceived and designed the study. VY collected and analysed the data, interpreted the findings, and drafted the manuscript. CU, EA and MT contributed significantly to revising the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded by Canadian Institutes of Health Research grants MOP-119541 and MOP-142379 to MT and a Frederick Banting and Charles Best Canada Graduate Scholarships Doctoral Award awarded to VY.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Simeon Wong, Rachel Leung, MyLoi Huynh-Silveira, Julie Lu Patranoiu, Rianne Hastie Adams, Rachael Lyon, Katharine Coons, Marc Lalancette, Tammy Rayner and Ruth Weiss for their help in recruitment, data collection and/or analyses. We are also very grateful to all individuals who participated in this study.

References

1. Demetriou EA, Lampit A, Quintana DS, Naismith SL, Song YJC, Pye JE, et al. Autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis of executive function. Mol Psychiatry (2018) 23:1198–204. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.75

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Pellicano E. The development of executive function in autism. Autism Res Treat (2012) 2012:146132. doi: 10.1155/2012/146132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Wallace GL, Kenworthy L, Pugliese CE, Popal HS, White EI, Brodsky E, et al. Real-world executive functions in adults with autism spectrum disorder: Profiles of impairment and associations with adaptive functioning and co-morbid anxiety and depression. J Autism Dev Disord (2016) 46:1071–83. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2655-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hill EL. Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends Cognit Sci (2004) 8:26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Baddeley A. Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annu Rev Psychol (2012) 63:1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. D’Esposito M. From cognitive to neural models of working memory. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci (2007) 362:761–72. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Aben B, Stapert S, Blokland A. About the distinction between working memory and short-term memory. Front Psychol (2012) 3:301. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Cowan N. What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? Prog Brain Res (2008) 169:323–38. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)00020-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Bowler DM, Poirier M, Martin JS, Gaigg SB. Nonverbal short-term serial order memory in autism spectrum disorder. J Abnorm Psychol (2016) 125:886–93. doi: 10.1037/abn0000203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Williams DL, Goldstein G, Minshew NJ. Impaired memory for faces and social scenes in autism: clinical implications of memory dysfunction. Arch Clin Neuropsychol (2005) 20:1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2002.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Desaunay P, Briant AR, Bowler DM, Ring M, Gérardin P, Baleyte J-M, et al. Memory in autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis of experimental studies. Psychol Bull (2020) 146:377–410. doi: 10.1037/bul0000225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Poirier M, Martin JS, Gaigg SB, Bowler DM. Short-term memory in autism spectrum disorder. J Abnorm Psychol (2011) 120:247–52. doi: 10.1037/a0022298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Wilhelm O, Hildebrandt A, Oberauer K. What is working memory capacity, and how can we measure it? Front Psychol (2013) 4:433. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Conway ARA, Kane MJ, Engle RW. Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends Cognit Sci (2003) 7:547–52. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Knight C, Stegmann Z. Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Appl Cognit Psychol (2004) 18:1–16. doi: 10.1002/acp.934

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Alloway TP, Alloway RG. Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. J Exp Child Psychol (2010) 106:20–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Bull R, Espy KA, Wiebe SA. Short-Term Memory, Working Memory, and Executive Functioning in Preschoolers: Longitudinal Predictors of Mathematical Achievement at Age 7 Years. Dev Neuropsychol (2008) 33:205–28. doi: 10.1080/87565640801982312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Murrihy C, Bailey M, Roodenburg J. Psychomotor Ability and Short-term Memory, and Reading and Mathematics Achievement in Children. Arch Clin Neuropsychol (2017) 32:618–30. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acx033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Tse VWS, Crabtree J, Islam S, Stott J. Comparing intellectual and memory abilities of older autistic adults with typically developing older adults using WAIS-IV and WMS-IV. J Autism Dev Disord (2019) 49:4123–33. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04122-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Wang Y, Zhang Y, Liu L, Cui J, Wang J, Shum DHK, et al. A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychol Rev (2017) 27:46–61. doi: 10.1007/s11065-016-9336-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Chien Y-L, Gau SS-F, Shang C-Y, Chiu Y-N, Tsai W-C, Wu Y-Y. Visual memory and sustained attention impairment in youths with autism spectrum disorders. Psychol Med (2015) 45:2263–73. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714003201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Williams DL, Goldstein G, Carpenter PA, Minshew NJ. Verbal and spatial working memory in autism. J Autism Dev Disord (2005) 35:747–56. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0021-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Zinke K, Fries E, Altgassen M, Kirschbaum C, Dettenborn L, Kliegel M. Visuospatial Short-Term Memory Explains Deficits in Tower Task Planning in High-Functioning Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Child Neuropsychol (2010) 16:229–41. doi: 10.1080/09297040903559648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Habib A, Harris L, Pollick F, Melville C. A meta-analysis of working memory in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. PloS One (2019) 14:e0216198. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0216198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Barendse EM, Schreuder LJ, Thoonen G, Hendriks MPH, Kessels RPC, Backes WH, et al. Working memory network alterations in high-functioning adolescents with an autism spectrum disorder. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2018) 72:73–83. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Koshino H, Kana RK, Keller TA, Cherkassky VL, Minshew NJ, Just MA. fMRI investigation of working memory for faces in autism: Visual coding and underconnectivity with frontal areas. Cereb Cortex (2008) 18:289–300. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Urbain CM, Pang EW, Taylor MJ. Atypical spatiotemporal signatures of working memory brain processes in autism. Transl Psychiatry (2015) 5:e617. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Silk TJ, Rinehart N, Bradshaw JL, Tonge B, Egan G, O’Boyle MW, et al. Visuospatial processing and the function of prefrontal-parietal networks in autism spectrum disorders: A functional MRI study. Am J Psychiatry (2006) 163:1440–3. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.8.1440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Braden BB, Smith CJ, Thompson A, Glaspy TK, Wood E, Vatsa D, et al. Executive function and functional and structural brain differences in middle-age adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res (2017) 10:1945–59. doi: 10.1002/aur.1842

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Vogan VM, Francis KE, Morgan BR, Smith ML, Taylor MJ. Load matters: Neural correlates of verbal working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodev Disord (2018) 10:19. doi: 10.1186/s11689-018-9236-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Koshino H, Carpenter PA, Minshew NJ, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Just MA. Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in high-functioning autism. Neuroimage (2005) 24:810–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Urbain C, Vogan VM, Ye AX, Pang EW, Doesburg SM, Taylor MJ. Desynchronization of fronto-temporal networks during working memory processing in autism. Hum Brain Mapp (2016) 37:153–64. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp (2005) 25:46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Rottschy C, Langner R, Dogan I, Reetz K, Laird AR, Schulz JB, et al. Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. Neuroimage (2012) 60:830–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Yaple ZA, Stevens WD, Arsalidou M. Meta-analyses of the n-back working memory task: fMRI evidence of age-related changes in prefrontal cortex involvement across the adult lifespan. Neuroimage (2019) 196:16–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Mencarelli L, Neri F, Momi D, Menardi A, Rossi S, Rossi A, et al. Stimuli, presentation modality, and load-specific brain activity patterns during n-back task. Hum Brain Mapp (2019) 40:3810–31. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Majerus S, D’Argembeau A, Martinez Perez T, Belayachi S, Van der Linden M, Collette F, et al. The Commonality of Neural Networks for Verbal and Visual Short-term Memory. J Cognit Neurosci (2010) 22:2570–93. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Babiloni C, Babiloni F, Carducci F, Cincotti F, Vecchio F, Cola B, et al. Functional Frontoparietal Connectivity During Short-Term Memory as Revealed by High-Resolution EEG Coherence Analysis. Behav Neurosci (2004) 118:687–97. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.4.687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Cashdollar N, Malecki U, Rugg-Gunn FJ, Duncan JS, Lavie N, Duzel E. Hippocampus-dependent and -independent theta-networks of active maintenance. Proc Natl Acad Sci (2009) 106:20493–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904823106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci (2002) 3:201–15. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Marek S, Dosenbach NUF. The frontoparietal network: Function, electrophysiology, and importance of individual precision mapping. Dialogues Clin Neurosci (2018) 20:133–40. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2018.20.2/smarek

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Fair DA, Dosenbach NUF, Church JA, Cohen AL, Brahmbhatt S, Miezin FM, et al. Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2007) 104:13507–12. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705843104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Harding IH, Harrison BJ, Breakspear M, Pantelis C, Yücel M. Cortical representations of cognitive control and working memory are dependent yet non-interacting. Cereb Cortex (2016) 26:557–65. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Dosenbach NUF, Fair DA, Miezin FM, Cohen AL, Wenger KK, Dosenbach RAT, et al. Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2007) 104:11073–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704320104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Becke A, Müller N, Vellage A, Schoenfeld MA, Hopf J-M. Neural sources of visual working memory maintenance in human parietal and ventral extrastriate visual cortex. Neuroimage (2015) 110:78–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Gazzaley A, Rissman J, D’Esposito M. Functional connectivity during working memory maintenance. Cognit Affect Behav Neurosci (2004) 4:580–99. doi: 10.3758/cabn.4.4.580

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Jonides J, Schumacher EH, Smith EE, Koeppe RA, Awh E, Reuter-Lorenz PA, et al. The role of parietal cortex in verbal working memory. J Neurosci (1998) 18:5026–34. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-13-05026.1998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ragland JD, Turetsky BI, Gur RC, Gunning-Dixon F, Turner T, Schroeder L, et al. Working memory for complex figures: An fMRI comparison of letter and fractal n-back tasks. Neuropsychology (2002) 16:370–9. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.16.3.370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Robitaille N, Marois R, Todd J, Grimault S, Cheyne D, Jolicœur P. Distinguishing between lateralized and nonlateralized brain activity associated with visual short-term memory: fMRI, MEG, and EEG evidence from the same observers. Neuroimage (2010) 53:1334–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Christophel TB, Hebart MN, Haynes J-D. Decoding the Contents of Visual Short-Term Memory from Human Visual and Parietal Cortex. J Neurosci (2012) 32:12983–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0184-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Todd JJ, Marois R. Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature (2004) 428:751–4. doi: 10.1038/nature02466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Fiez J, Raife E, Balota D, Schwarz J, Raichle M, Petersen S. A positron emission tomography study of the short-term maintenance of verbal information. J Neurosci (1996) 16:808–22. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-02-00808.1996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Petrides M. Lateral prefrontal cortex: Architectonic and functional organization. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci (2005) 360:781–95. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1631

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Curtis CE, D’Esposito M. Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends Cognit Sci (2003) 7:415–23. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00197-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Courtney SM, Petit L, Haxby JV, Ungerleider LG. The role of prefrontal cortex in working memory: Examining the contents of consciousness. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci (1998) 353:1819–28. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0334

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Serences JT. Neural mechanisms of information storage in visual short-term memory. Vision Res (2016) 128:53–67. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2016.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. D’Esposito M, Postle BR, Rypma B. Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: Evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Exp Brain Res (2000) 133:3–11. doi: 10.1007/s002210000395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Ranganath C, Johnson MK, D’Esposito M. Prefrontal activity associated with working memory and episodic long-term memory. Neuropsychologia (2003) 41:378–89. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00169-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Veltman DJ, Rombouts SARB, Dolan RJ. Maintenance versus manipulation in verbal working memory revisited: An fMRI study. Neuroimage (2003) 18:247–56. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00049-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Rowe JB, Toni I, Josephs O, Frackowiak RSJ, Passingham RE. The prefrontal cortex: Response selection or maintenance within working memory? Science (2000) 288:1656–60. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5471.1656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Yeung MK, Lee TL, Chan AS. Right-lateralized frontal activation underlies successful updating of verbal working memory in adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychol (2019) 148:107743. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.107743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Luna B, Minshew NJ, Garver KE, Lazar NA, Thulborn KR, Eddy WF, et al. Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: An fMRI study of spatial working memory. Neurology (2002) 59:834–40. doi: 10.1212/WNL.59.6.834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Vogan VM, Morgan BR, Lou SM, Taylor MJ. Functional changes during visuo-spatial working memory in autism spectrum disorder: 2-year longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Autism (2019) 23:639–52. doi: 10.1177/1362361318766572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Rahko JS, Vuontela VA, Carlson S, Nikkinen J, Hurtig TM, Kuusikko-Gauffin S, et al. Attention and working memory in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: A functional MRI study. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2016) 47:503–17. doi: 10.1007/s10578-015-0583-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Cohen JR, D’Esposito M. The segregation and integration of distinct brain networks and their relationship to cognition. J Neurosci (2016) 36:12083–94. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2965-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Finc K, Bonna K, Lewandowska M, Wolak T, Nikadon J, Dreszer J, et al. Transition of the functional brain network related to increasing cognitive demands. Hum Brain Mapp (2017) 38:3659–74. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Cole MW, Reynolds JR, Power JD, Repovs G, Anticevic A, Braver TS. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nat Neurosci (2013) 16:1348–55. doi: 10.1038/nn.3470

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Spreng RN, Stevens WD, Chamberlain JP, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL. Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. Neuroimage (2010) 53:303–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Dixon ML, De La Vega A, Mills C, Andrews-Hanna J, Spreng RN, Cole MW, et al. Heterogeneity within the frontoparietal control network and its relationship to the default and dorsal attention networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2018) 115:E1598–607. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1715766115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Cocchi L, Zalesky A, Fornito A, Mattingley JB. Dynamic cooperation and competition between brain systems during cognitive control. Trends Cognit Sci (2013) 17:493–501. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Belmonte MK, Allen G, Beckel-Mitchener A, Boulanger LM, Carper RA, Webb SJ. Autism and abnormal development of brain connectivity. J Neurosci (2004) 24:9228–31. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3340-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. O’Reilly C, Lewis JD, Elsabbagh M. Is functional brain connectivity atypical in autism? A systematic review of EEG and MEG studies. PloS One (2017) 12:e0175870. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Kessler K, Seymour RA, Rippon G. Brain oscillations and connectivity in autism spectrum disorders (ASD): New approaches to methodology, measurement and modelling. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2016) 71:601–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Di Martino A, Yan C-G, Li Q, Denio E, Castellanos FX, Alaerts K, et al. The autism brain imaging data exchange: Towards a large-scale evaluation of the intrinsic brain architecture in autism. Mol Psychiatry (2014) 19:659–67. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Kana RK, Libero LE, Moore MS. Disrupted cortical connectivity theory as an explanatory model for autism spectrum disorders. Phys Life Rev (2011) 8:410–37. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2011.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Geschwind DH, Levitt P. Autism spectrum disorders: Developmental disconnection syndromes. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2007) 17:103–11. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Palva JM, Monto S, Kulashekhar S, Palva S. Neuronal synchrony reveals working memory networks and predicts individual memory capacity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2010) 107:7580–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913113107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Roux F, Uhlhaas PJ. Working memory and neural oscillations: α-γ versus θ-γ codes for distinct WM information? Trends Cognit Sci (2014) 18:16–25. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Johnson EL, Dewar CD, Solbakk A-K, Endestad T, Meling TR, Knight RT. Bidirectional frontoparietal oscillatory systems support working memory. Curr Biol (2017) 27:1829–35. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Sarnthein J, Petsche H, Rappelsberger P, Shaw GL, von Stein A. Synchronization between prefrontal and posterior association cortex during human working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (1998) 95:7092–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.12.7092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Klimesch W, Freunberger R, Sauseng P. Oscillatory mechanisms of process binding in memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2010) 34:1002–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Liebe S, Hoerzer GM, Logothetis NK, Rainer G. Theta coupling between V4 and prefrontal cortex predicts visual short-term memory performance. Nat Neurosci (2012) 15:456–62. doi: 10.1038/nn.3038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Freunberger R, Klimesch W, Griesmayr B, Sauseng P, Gruber W. Alpha phase coupling reflects object recognition. Neuroimage (2008) 42:928–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Payne L, Kounios J. Coherent oscillatory networks supporting short-term memory retention. Brain Res (2009) 1247:126–32. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.09.095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Doesburg SM, Herdman AT, Ribary U, Cheung T, Moiseev A, Weinberg H, et al. Long-range synchronization and local desynchronization of alpha oscillations during visual short-term memory retention in children. Exp Brain Res (2010) 201:719–27. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2086-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Nyrenius J, Billstedt E. The functional impact of cognition in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Nord J Psychiatry (2020) 74:220–5. doi: 10.1080/08039488.2019.1694698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Carpentieri S, Morgan SB. Adaptive and intellectual functioning in autistic and nonautistic retarded children. J Autism Dev Disord (1996) 26:611–20. doi: 10.1007/BF02172350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Kirchner WK. Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. J Exp Psychol (1958) 55:352–8. doi: 10.1037/h0043688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Kundu B, Chang J-Y, Postle BR, Van Veen BD. Context-specific differences in fronto-parieto-occipital effective connectivity during short-term memory maintenance. Neuroimage (2015) 114:320–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Tallon-Baudry C. Oscillatory synchrony and human visual cognition. J Physiol (2003) 97:355–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2003.09.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Wilsch A, Henry MJ, Herrmann B, Herrmann CS, Obleser J. Temporal Expectation Modulates the Cortical Dynamics of Short-Term Memory. J Neurosci (2018) 38:7428–39. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2928-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Schack B, Klimesch W. Frequency characteristics of evoked and oscillatory electroencephalic activity in a human memory scanning task. Neurosci Lett (2002) 331:107–10. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00846-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Düzel E, Penny WD, Burgess N. Brain oscillations and memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2010) 20:143–9. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Salazar RF, Dotson NM, Bressler SL, Gray CM. Content-specific fronto-parietal synchronization during visual working memory. Science (2012) 338:1097–101. doi: 10.1126/science.1224000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Daume J, Graetz S, Gruber T, Engel AK, Friese U. Cognitive control during audiovisual working memory engages frontotemporal theta-band interactions. Sci Rep (2017) 7:12585. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12511-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Dai Z, de Souza J, Lim J, Ho PM, Chen Y, Li J, et al. EEG cortical connectivity analysis of working memory reveals topological reorganization in theta and alpha bands. Front Hum Neurosci (2017) 11:237. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Bressler SL, Richter CG. Interareal oscillatory synchronization in top-down neocortical processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2015) 31:62–6. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Kopell N, Ermentrout GB, Whittington MA, Traub RD. Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2000) 97:1867–72. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.4.1867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. von Stein A, Chiang C, König P. Top-down processing mediated by interareal synchronization. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2000) 97:14748–53. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.26.14748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Johnson EL, King-Stephens D, Weber PB, Laxer KD, Lin JJ, Knight RT. Spectral imprints of working memory for everyday associations in the frontoparietal network. Front Syst Neurosci (2019) 12:65. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2018.00065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Murray JD, Jaramillo J, Wang X-J. Working memory and decision-making in a frontoparietal circuit model. J Neurosci (2017) 37:12167–86. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0343-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Sato J, Mossad SI, Wong SM, Hunt BAE, Dunkley BT, Lou SM, et al. Alpha keeps it together: Alpha oscillatory synchrony underlies working memory maintenance in young children. Dev Cognit Neurosci (2018) 34:114–23. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Doppelmayr M, Pecherstorfer T, Freunberger R, Hanslmayr S. EEG alpha synchronization and functional coupling during top-down processing in a working memory task. Hum Brain Mapp (2005) 26:148–55. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Crespo-Garcia M, Pinal D, Cantero JL, Díaz F, Zurrón M, Atienza M. Working memory processes are mediated by local and long-range synchronization of alpha oscillations. J Cognit Neurosci (2013) 25:1343–57. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Jensen O, Gelfand J, Kounios J, Lisman JE. Oscillations in the alpha band (9-12 Hz) increase with memory load during retention in a short-term memory task. Cereb Cortex (2002) 12:877–82. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.8.877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation (1999).

Google Scholar

107. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, Second Edition (WASI-II). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson (2011).

Google Scholar

108. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop SL. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2) Manual (Part I): Modules 1-4. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services (2012).

Google Scholar

109. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH Jr., Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord (2000) 30:205–23. doi: 10.1023/A:1005592401947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Roth RM, Isquith PK, Gioia GA. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Adult Version. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources (2005).

Google Scholar

111. Constantino JN, Gruber CP. Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services (2012).

Google Scholar

112. Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen J-M. FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci (2011) 2011:156869. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Nolte G. The magnetic lead field theorem in the quasi-static approximation and its use for magnetoenchephalography forward calculation in realistic volume conductors. Phys Med Biol (2003) 48:3637–52. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/48/22/002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage (2002) 15:273–89. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Van Veen BD, Van Drongelen W, Yuchtman M, Suzuki A. Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering. IEEE Trans BioMed Eng (1997) 44:867–80. doi: 10.1109/10.623056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Breakspear M, Williams LM, Stam CJ. A novel method for the topographic analysis of neural activity reveals formation and dissolution of “dynamic cell assemblies.” J Comput Neurosci (2004) 16:49–68. doi: 10.1023/B:JCNS.0000004841.66897.7d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Tewarie P, Liuzzi L, O’Neill GC, Quinn AJ, Griffa A, Woolrich MW, et al. Tracking dynamic brain networks using high temporal resolution MEG measures of functional connectivity. Neuroimage (2019) 200:38–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Zalesky A, Fornito A, Bullmore ET. Network-based statistic: Identifying differences in brain networks. Neuroimage (2010) 53:1197–207. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Xia M, Wang J, He Y. BrainNet Viewer: A network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PloS One (2013) 8:e68910. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Koelewijn L, Lancaster TM, Linden D, Dima DC, Routley BC, Magazzini L, et al. Oscillatory hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity in young APOE-ε4 carriers and hypoconnectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. Elife (2019) 8:e36011. doi: 10.7554/eLife.36011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Lever AG, Werkle-Bergner M, Brandmaier AM, Ridderinkhof KR, Geurts HM. Atypical working memory decline across the adult lifespan in autism spectrum disorder? J Abnorm Psychol (2015) 124:1014–26. doi: 10.1037/abn0000108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Haigh SM, Walsh JA, Mazefsky CA, Minshew NJ, Eack SM. Processing speed is impaired in adults with autism spectrum disorder, and relates to social communication abilities. J Autism Dev Disord (2018) 48:2653–62. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3515-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Oliveras-Rentas RE, Kenworthy L, Roberson RB, Martin A, Wallace GL. WISC-IV profile in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: Impaired processing speed is associated with increased autism communication symptoms and decreased adaptive communication abilities. J Autism Dev Disord (2012) 42:655–64. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1289-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Travers BG, Bigler ED, Tromp DPM, Adluru N, Froehlich AL, Ennis C, et al. Longitudinal processing speed impairments in males with autism and the effects of white matter microstructure. Neuropsychologia (2014) 53:137–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Hedvall Å, Fernell E, Holm A, Åsberg Johnels J, Gillberg C, Billstedt E. Autism, processing speed, and adaptive functioning in preschool children. Sci World J (2013) 2013:158263. doi: 10.1155/2013/158263

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL. Learning, attention, writing, and processing speed in typical children and children with ADHD, autism, anxiety, depression, and oppositional-defiant disorder. Child Neuropsychol (2007) 13:469–93. doi: 10.1080/09297040601112773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Luna B, Doll SK, Hegedus SJ, Minshew NJ, Sweeney JA. Maturation of executive function in autism. Biol Psychiatry (2007) 61:474–81. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.02.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Cardillo R, Lanfranchi S, Mammarella IC. A cross-task comparison on visuospatial processing in autism spectrum disorders. Autism (2020) 24:765–79. doi: 10.1177/1362361319888341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Wallace GL, Anderson M, Happé F. Brief report: Information processing speed is intact in autism but not correlated with measured intelligence. J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:809–14. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0684-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL. WISC-IV and WIAT-II profiles in children with high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:428–39. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0410-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Fried R, Joshi G, Bhide P, Pope A, Galdo M, Koster A, et al. A study of the neuropsychological correlates in adults with high functioning autism spectrum disorders. Acta Neuropsychiatr (2016) 28:286–95. doi: 10.1017/neu.2016.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Braaten EB, Ward AK, Forchelli G, Vuijk PJ, Cook NE, McGuinness P, et al. Characteristics of child psychiatric outpatients with slow processing speed and potential mechanisms of academic impact. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2020). doi: 10.1007/s00787-019-01455-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Tsukiura T, Fujii T, Takahashi T, Xiao R, Inase M, Iijima T, et al. Neuroanatomical discrimination between manipulating and maintaining processes involved in verbal working memory; a functional MRI study. Cognit Brain Res (2001) 11:13–21. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(00)00059-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Paulesu E, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ. The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature (1993) 362:342–5. doi: 10.1038/362342a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Sheremata SL, Somers DC, Shomstein S. Visual short-term memory activity in parietal lobe reflects cognitive processes beyond attentional selection. J Neurosci (2018) 38:1511–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1716-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Bettencourt KC, Xu Y. Decoding the content of visual short-term memory under distraction in occipital and parietal areas. Nat Neurosci (2016) 19:150–7. doi: 10.1038/nn.4174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Nee DE, Brown JW, Askren MK, Berman MG, Demiralp E, Krawitz A, et al. A meta-analysis of executive components of working memory. Cereb Cortex (2013) 23:264–82. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Berryhill ME. Insights from neuropsychology: Pinpointing the role of the posterior parietal cortex in episodic and working memory. Front Integr Neurosci (2012) 6:31. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Andersen RA, Essick GK, Siegel RM. Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. Science (1985) 230:456–8. doi: 10.1126/science.4048942

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Alain C, Shen D, Yu H, Grady C. Dissociable memory- and response-related activity in parietal cortex during auditory spatial working memory. Front Psychol (2010) 1:202. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Passaro AD, Elmore LC, Ellmore TM, Leising KJ, Papanicolaou AC, Wright AA. Explorations of object and location memory using fMRI. Front Behav Neurosci (2013) 7:105. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Rottschy C, Caspers S, Roski C, Reetz K, Dogan I, Schulz JB, et al. Differentiated parietal connectivity of frontal regions for “what” and “where” memory. Brain Struct Funct (2013) 218:1551–67. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0476-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Courtney SM, Ungerleider LG, Keil K, Haxby JV. Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. Cereb Cortex (1996) 6:39–49. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.1.39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Thomas KM, King SW, Franzen PL, Welsh TF, Berkowitz AL, Noll DC, et al. A developmental functional MRI study of spatial working memory. Neuroimage (1999) 10:327–38. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Harrison A, Jolicoeur P, Marois R. “What” and “Where” in the Intraparietal Sulcus: An fMRI Study of Object Identity and Location in Visual Short-Term Memory. Cereb Cortex (2010) 20:2478–85. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Olson IR, Berryhill M. Some surprising findings on the involvement of the parietal lobe in human memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem (2009) 91:155–65. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Duggirala SX, Saharan S, Raghunathan P, Mandal PK. Stimulus-dependent modulation of working memory for identity monitoring: A functional MRI study. Brain Cognit (2016) 102:55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Finke K, Bublak P, Zihl J. Visual spatial and visual pattern working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for a differential role of left and right dorsal visual brain. Neuropsychologia (2006) 44:649–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Xu Y, Chun MM. Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term memory for objects. Nature (2006) 440:91–5. doi: 10.1038/nature04262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Konstantinou N, Constantinidou F, Kanai R. Discrete capacity limits and neuroanatomical correlates of visual short-term memory for objects and spatial locations. Hum Brain Mapp (2017) 38:767–78. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct (2010) 214:655–67. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V. A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2008) 105:12569–74. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800005105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Eckert MA, Menon V, Walczak A, Ahlstrom J, Denslow S, Horwitz A, et al. At the heart of the ventral attention system: The right anterior insula. Hum Brain Mapp (2009) 30:2530–41. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Uddin LQ, Kinnison J, Pessoa L, Anderson ML. Beyond the tripartite cognition-emotion-interoception model of the human insular cortex. J Cognit Neurosci (2014) 26:16–27. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00462

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Cauda F, Costa T, Torta DME, Sacco K, D’Agata F, Duca S, et al. Meta-analytic clustering of the insular cortex: Characterizing the meta-analytic connectivity of the insula when involved in active tasks. Neuroimage (2012) 62:343–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. D’Esposito M, Postle BR. The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annu Rev Psychol (2015) 66:115–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

157. Barbey AK, Koenigs M, Grafman J. Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex (2013) 49:1195–205. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Wager TD, Smith EE. Neuroimaging studies of working memory: A meta-analysis. Cognit Affect Behav Neurosci (2003) 3:255–74. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.4.255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. O’Reilly RC, Braver TS, Cohen JD. A biologically based computational model of working memory. In: Miyake A, Shah P, editors. Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press (1999). p. 375–411. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139174909.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

160. Feredoes E, Heinen K, Weiskopf N, Ruff C, Driver J. Causal evidence for frontal involvement in memory target maintenance by posterior brain areas during distracter interference of visual working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2011) 108:17510–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1106439108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

161. Edin F, Klingberg T, Johansson P, McNab F, Tegnér J, Compte A. Mechanism for top-down control of working memory capacity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2009) 106:6802–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901894106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

162. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. The precuneus: A review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain (2006) 129:564–83. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. Zhang S, Li CR. Functional connectivity mapping of the human precuneus by resting state fMRI. Neuroimage (2012) 59:3548–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

164. Margulies DS, Vincent JL, Kelly C, Lohmann G, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, et al. Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2009) 106:20069–74. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905314106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Mahayana IT, Tcheang L, Chen C-Y, Juan C-H, Muggleton NG. The precuneus and visuospatial attention in near and far space: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Brain Stimul (2014) 7:673–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

166. Selemon LD, Goldman-Rakic PS. Common cortical and subcortical targets of the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus monkey: Evidence for a distributed neural network subserving spatially guided behavior. J Neurosci (1988) 8:4049–68. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-11-04049.1988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

167. Leichnetz GR. Connections of the medial posterior parietal cortex (area 7m) in the monkey. Anat Rec (2001) 263:215–36. doi: 10.1002/ar.1082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

168. Suchan B, Yágüez L, Wunderlich G, Canavan AGM, Herzog H, Tellmann L, et al. Hemispheric dissociation of visual-pattern processing and visual rotation. Behav Brain Res (2002) 136:533–44. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00204-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

169. Fletcher PC, Frith CD, Baker SC, Shallice T, Frackowiak RSJ, Dolan RJ. The mind’s eye–precuneus activation in memory-related imagery. Neuroimage (1995) 2:195–200. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1995.1025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

170. Sugiura M, Shah NJ, Zilles K, Fink GR. Cortical representations of personally familiar objects and places: Functional organization of the human posterior cingulate cortex. J Cognit Neurosci (2005) 17:183–98. doi: 10.1162/0898929053124956

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

171. Nyberg L, Dahlin E, Stigsdotter Neely A, Bäckman L. Neural correlates of variable working memory load across adult age and skill: Dissociative patterns within the fronto-parietal network. Scand J Psychol (2009) 50:41–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00678.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

172. Robitaille N, Grimault S, Jolicœur P. Bilateral parietal and contralateral responses during maintenance of unilaterally encoded objects in visual short-term memory: Evidence from magnetoencephalography. Psychophysiology (2009) 46:1090–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00837.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

173. Linden DEJ, Bittner RA, Muckli L, Waltz JA, Kriegeskorte N, Goebel R, et al. Cortical capacity constraints for visual working memory: dissociation of fMRI load effects in a fronto-parietal network. Neuroimage (2003) 20:1518–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

174. Vogan VM, Morgan BR, Powell TL, Smith ML, Taylor MJ. The neurodevelopmental differences of increasing verbal working memory demand in children and adults. Dev Cognit Neurosci (2016) 17:19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.10.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

175. Rypma B, Prabhakaran V, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JDE. Load-dependent roles of frontal brain regions in the maintenance of working memory. Neuroimage (1999) 9:216–26. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

176. Gould RL, Brown RG, Owen AM, Ffytche DH, Howard RJ. fMRI BOLD response to increasing task difficulty during successful paired associates learning. Neuroimage (2003) 20:1006–19. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00365-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

177. Höller-Wallscheid MS, Thier P, Pomper JK, Lindner A. Bilateral recruitment of prefrontal cortex in working memory is associated with task demand but not with age. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2017) 114:E830–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1601983114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

178. Heinzel S, Lorenz RC, Duong Q-L, Rapp MA, Deserno L. Prefrontal-parietal effective connectivity during working memory in older adults. Neurobiol Aging (2017) 57:18–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

179. Wianda E, Ross B. The roles of alpha oscillation in working memory retention. Brain Behav (2019) 9:e01263. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

180. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: One decade on. Trends Cognit Sci (2014) 18:177–85. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

181. Vidal J, Mills T, Pang EW, Taylor MJ. Response inhibition in adults and teenagers: Spatiotemporal differences in the prefrontal cortex. Brain Cognit (2012) 79:49–59. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.12.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

182. Vara AS, Pang EW, Vidal J, Anagnostou E, Taylor MJ. Neural mechanisms of inhibitory control continue to mature in adolescence. Dev Cognit Neurosci (2014) 10:129–39. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

183. Rubia K, Smith AB, Brammer MJ, Taylor E. Right inferior prefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesial prefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection. Neuroimage (2003) 20:351–8. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00275-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

184. Forstmann BU, Jahfari S, Scholte HS, Wolfensteller U, van den Wildenberg WPM, Ridderinkhof KR. Function and structure of the right inferior frontal cortex predict individual differences in response inhibition: A model-based approach. J Neurosci (2008) 28:9790–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1465-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

185. Anderson MC, Ochsner KN, Kuhl B, Cooper J, Robertson E, Gabrieli SW, et al. Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science (2004) 303:232–5. doi: 10.1126/science.1089504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

186. Emch M, von Bastian CC, Koch K. Neural correlates of verbal working memory: An fMRI meta-analysis. Front Hum Neurosci (2019) 13:180:180. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

187. Bomyea J, Taylor CT, Spadoni AD, Simmons AN. Neural mechanisms of interference control in working memory capacity. Hum Brain Mapp (2018) 39:772–82. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

188. Geurts HM, van den Bergh SFWM, Ruzzano L. Prepotent response inhibition and interference control in autism spectrum disorders: Two meta-analyses. Autism Res (2014) 7:407–20. doi: 10.1002/aur.1369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

189. Adams NC, Jarrold C. Inhibition in autism: Children with autism have difficulty inhibiting irrelevant distractors but not prepotent responses. J Autism Dev Disord (2012) 42:1052–63. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1345-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

190. Christ SE, Kester LE, Bodner KE, Miles JH. Evidence for selective inhibitory impairment in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology (2011) 25:690–701. doi: 10.1037/a0024256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

191. Lever AG, Ridderinkhof KR, Marsman M, Geurts HM. Reactive and proactive interference control in adults with autism spectrum disorder across the lifespan. Dev Psychol (2017) 53:379–95. doi: 10.1037/dev0000219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

192. Ozonoff S, Strayer DL. Inhibitory function in nonretarded children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord (1997) 27:59–77. doi: 10.1023/a:1025821222046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

193. Sauseng P, Griesmayr B, Freunberger R, Klimesch W. Control mechanisms in working memory: A possible function of EEG theta oscillations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2010) 34:1015–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

194. Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Freunberger R, Pecherstorfer T, Hanslmayr S, Doppelmayr M. Relevance of EEG alpha and theta oscillations during task switching. Exp Brain Res (2006) 170:295–301. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0211-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

195. Cavanagh JF, Frank MJ. Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cognit Sci (2014) 18:414–21. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

196. Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Doppelmayr M, Hanslmayr S, Schabus M, Gruber WR. Theta coupling in the human electroencephalogram during a working memory task. Neurosci Lett (2004) 354:123–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

197. Picci G, Gotts SJ, Scherf KS. A theoretical rut: Revisiting and critically evaluating the generalized under/over-connectivity hypothesis of autism. Dev Sci (2016) 19:524–49. doi: 10.1111/desc.12467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

198. Just MA, Keller TA, Malave VL, Kana RK, Varma S. Autism as a neural systems disorder: A theory of frontal-posterior underconnectivity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2012) 36:1292–313. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

199. Vissers ME, Cohen MX, Geurts HM. Brain connectivity and high functioning autism: A promising path of research that needs refined models, methodological convergence, and stronger behavioral links. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2012) 36:604–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

200. Holz EM, Glennon M, Prendergast K, Sauseng P. Theta–gamma phase synchronization during memory matching in visual working memory. Neuroimage (2010) 52:326–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

201. Polanía R, Nitsche MA, Korman C, Batsikadze G, Paulus W. The Importance of Timing in Segregated Theta Phase-Coupling for Cognitive Performance. Curr Biol (2012) 22:1314–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

202. Jacobs J, Hwang G, Curran T, Kahana MJ. EEG oscillations and recognition memory: Theta correlates of memory retrieval and decision making. Neuroimage (2006) 32:978–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

203. Schack B, Klimesch W, Sauseng P. Phase synchronization between theta and upper alpha oscillations in a working memory task. Int J Psychophysiol (2005) 57:105–14. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

204. Popov T, Popova P, Harkotte M, Awiszus B, Rockstroh B, Miller GA. Cross-frequency interactions between frontal theta and posterior alpha control mechanisms foster working memory. Neuroimage (2018) 181:728–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

205. Klimesch W, Freunberger R, Sauseng P, Gruber W. A short review of slow phase synchronization and memory: Evidence for control processes in different memory systems? Brain Res (2008) 1235:31–44. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.06.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

206. Hunt BAE, Wong SM, Vandewouw MM, Brookes MJ, Dunkley BT, Taylor MJ. Spatial and spectral trajectories in typical neurodevelopment from childhood to middle age. Netw Neurosci (2019) 3:497–520. doi: 10.1162/netn_a_00077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

207. Vogan VM, Morgan BR, Lee W, Powell TL, Smith ML, Taylor MJ. The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: Effects of cognitive load. J Neurodev Disord (2014) 6:19. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-6-19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism, connectivity, working memory, theta, alpha, maintenance, recognition, MEG

Citation: Yuk V, Urbain C, Anagnostou E and Taylor MJ (2020) Frontoparietal Network Connectivity During an N-Back Task in Adults With Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 11:551808. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.551808

Received: 14 April 2020; Accepted: 13 August 2020;
Published: 09 September 2020.

Edited by:

Peter Uhlhaas, University of Glasgow, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Satu Palva, University of Helsinki, Finland
Peter G. Enticott, Deakin University, Australia

Copyright © 2020 Yuk, Urbain, Anagnostou and Taylor. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Veronica Yuk, veronica.yuk@mail.utoronto.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.