Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-02T04:11:42.271Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 August 2009

Jutta Heckhausen
Affiliation:
University of California, Irvine
Heinz Heckhausen
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Psychologische Forschung, Munich
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Motivation and Action , pp. 445 - 500
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A. P., & Midden, C. (1999). To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviors. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 971–979.3.0.CO;2-A>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abel, T. M. (1938). Neuro-circularity reaction and the recall of unfinished and completed tasks. Journal of Psychology, 6, 377–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abele, A. E., Andrä, M. S., & Schute, T. (1999). Wer hat nach dem Hochschulexamen schnell eine Stelle? Erste Ergebnisse der Erlanger Längsschnittstudie (BELA-E) [Who enters a job quickly after graduating? Results of a longitudinal study (BELA-E)]. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 43, 95–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96, 358–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ach, N. (1905). Über die Willenstätigkeit und das Denken [On the activity of will and on thinking]. Göttingen, Germany: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.Google Scholar
Ach, N. (1910). Über den Willensakt und das Temperament [On acts of will and temperament]. Leipzig, Germany: Quelle & Meyer.Google Scholar
Ach, N. (1935). Analyse des Willens [Analysis of the will]. In Abderhalden, E. (Ed.), Handbuch der biologischen Arbeitsmethoden (Vol. 6, Part E 460). Berlin, Germany: Urban & Schwarzberg.Google Scholar
Achtziger, A. (2003). Kognitionspsychologische Aspekte der willentlichen Stereotypkontrolle [Cognitive aspects of voluntary stereotype control]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Konstanz, Germany.Google Scholar
Achtziger, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). Controling stereotypes and prejudice by implementation intentions. Paper presented at the Small Group Meeting of the EASP, Jena, Germany.Google Scholar
Achtziger, A., Rockstroh, B., Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2003, September). Neurophysiologische Korrelate verschiedener handlungsvorbereitender mentaler Zustände [Neurophysiological correlates of goal setting strategies]. Paper presented at the 9th meeting of the Fachgruppe Sozialpsychologie, Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Achtziger, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (in press). Implementation intentions and shielding goal striving from unwanted thoughts and feelings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.Google Scholar
Achtziger, A., Michalski, V., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (forthcoming). Processing stereotype-inconsistent information under cognitive load.
Achtziger, A., Moratti, S., Jaudas, A., Rockstroh, B., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (forthcoming). The control of automatic stereotype activation by means of implementation intentions in the EEG.
Adams, R. E., & Passman, R. H. (1979). Effects of visual and auditory aspects of mothers and stranger on the play and exploration of children. Developmental Psychology, 15(3), 269–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adler, A. (1922). Über den nervösen Charakter [The neurotic constitution] (3rd ed.). Munich, Germany: Bergmann.Google Scholar
Adler, D. L., & Kounin, J. S. (1939). Some factors operating at the moment of resumption of interrupted tasks. Journal of Psychology, 7, 255–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adolphs, R., & Tranel, D. (2000). Emotion recognition and the human amygdala. In Aggleton, J. P. (Ed.), The amygdala: A functional analysis (pp. 587–630). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Aellig, S. (2004). Über den Sinn des Unsinns. Flow-Erleben und Wohlbefinden als Anreize für autotelische Tätigkeiten [The sense in not being sensible: Flow experience and well-being as motivation for autotelic activities]. Münster, Germany: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., Connor, O' E., & Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32(3), 537–551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Affleck, G., Tennen, H., Croog, S., & Levine, S. (1987). Causal attribution, perceived benefits and morbidity after a heart attack: An 8 year study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 29–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainley, M. D. (1993). Styles of engagement with learning: Multidimensional assessment of their relationship with strategy use and school achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), 395–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. (1972). Attachment and dependency: A comparison. In Gewirtz, J. (Ed.), Attachment and dependency (pp. 251). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. (1979). Infant-mother attachment. American Psychologist, 34, 932–937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ainsworth, M. D., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration and separation: Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child Development, 41(1), 49–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. (1974). Infant mother attachment and social development: Socialisation as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In Richards, R. P. M. (Ed.), The integration of a child into a social world. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. N. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1969). The prediction of behavioral intentions in a choice situation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 5, 400–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alberch, P. (1980). Ontogenesis and morphological diversification. American Zoologist, 20, 653–667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albert, D. J., Jonik, R. H., & Walsh, M. L. (1992). Hormone-dependent aggression in male and female rats: Experiential, hormonal, and neural foundations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 16(2), 177–192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albert, D. J., Petrovic, D. M., Walsh, M. L., & Jonik, R. H. (1989). Medial accumbens lesions attenuate testosterone-dependent aggression in male rats. Physiology & Behavior, 46, 625–631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alker, H. A. (1972). Is personality situationally specific or intrapsychically consistent?Journal of Personality, 40, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allensbacher Markt- and Werbeträgeranalyse (AWA). (1995–2000) (2000). Berichtsband I. Markstrukturen [Technical reports I. Market structures]. Allensbach, Germany: Institut für Demoskopie.
Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser?Journal of Experimental Psychology, 108, 441–485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1988). Depressive realism: Four theoretical perspectives. In Alloy, L. B. (Ed.), Cognitive processes in depression (pp. 223–265). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1953). The trend in motivation theory. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 23, 107–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, G. W., & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait-names: A psycholexical study. Psychological Monographs, 47(211).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alper, T. G. (1946). Task-orientation vs. ego-orientation in learning and retention. American Journal of Psychology, 59, 236–248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alper, T. G. (1957). Predicting the direction of selective recall: Its relation to ego strength and n achievement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55, 149–165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altshuler, J. A., & Ruble, D. N. (1989). Developmental changes in children's awareness of strategies for coping with uncontrollable stress. Child Development, 60, 1337–1349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ames, C. (1978). Children's achievement attributions and self reinforcement: Effects of self concept and competitive reward structure. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 345–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ames, C., & Ames, R. (1984). Systems of student and teacher motivation: Toward a qualitative definition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 535–556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ames, C., Ames, R., & Felker, D. W. (1977). Effects of competitive reward structure and valence of outcome on children's achievement attributions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: Students' learning strategies and motivational processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(3), 260–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, C., & Berdahl, J. L. (2002). Examining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1362–1377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, C. A. (1983). Imagination and expectation: The effect of imagining behavioral scripts on personal intentions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 293–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition: Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Andersen, S. M., & Chen, S. U. (2002). The relational self: An interpersonal social-cognitive theory. Psychological Review, 109, 619–645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andresen, B. (1995). Risikobereitschaft (R): Der sechste Basisfaktor der Persönlichkeit: Konvergenz multivariater Studien und Konstruktexplikation [“Risk preference” (R) – The sixth basic factor of personality: Convergence of multivariate studies and construct explication]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 16, 210–236.Google Scholar
Andrews, J. D. W. (1967). The achievement motive and advancement in two types of organizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 163–168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angermeier, W. F., & Peters, M. (1973). Bedingte Reaktionen [Conditioned reactions]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angleitner, A., & Ostendorf, F. (1994). Temperament and the Big Five factors of personality. In Halverson, C. F., Kohnstamm, G. A., & Martin, R. P. (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 69–90). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (2003). The effects of mindset on behavior: Self-regulation in deliberative and implemental frames of mind. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 86–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, R. M. (1960). Emotion and personality: Vol. I: Psychological aspects, Vol. II: Neurological and psychological aspects. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1962). Performance expectancy as a determinant of actual performance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 178–183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asendorpf, J. B. (1984). Shyness, embarrassment, and self-presentation: A control theory approach. In Schwarzer, R. (Ed.), The self in anxiety, stress, and depression (pp. 109–114). Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
Asendorpf, J. B. (2004). Psychologie der Persönlichkeit [Psychology of personality]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asendorpf, J. B., Banse, R., & Mücke, D. (2002). Double dissociation between implicit and explicit personality self-concept: The case of shy behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 380–393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asendorpf, J. B., Weber, A., & Burkhardt, K. (1994). Zur Mehrdeutigkeit projektiver Testergebnisse: Motiv-Projektionen oder Thema-Sensitivität? [On the multiple meaning of projective tests: Motive projection or thematic sensitivity?]Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 15, 155–165.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1953). The achievement motive and recall of interrupted and completed tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 381–390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359–372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.). (1958a). Motives in fantasy, action, and society. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1958b). Towards experimental analysis of human motivation in terms of motives, expectancies and incentives. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 288–305). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1964a). An introduction to motivation. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1964b). Some neglected variables in contemporary conceptions of decision and performance. Psychological Reports, 14, 575–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1974a). Motivational determinants of intellective performance and cumulative achievement. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 389–410). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1974b). Strength of motivation and efficiency of performance. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 193–218). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1981). Studying personality in the context of an advanced motivational psychology. American Psychologist, 36, 171–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1987). Michigan studies of fear of failure. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 47–59). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. (1970). The dynamics of action. New York: Wiley.Google ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. A. (1978). Introduction to motivation (2nd ed.). New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., Bongort, K., & Price, L. H. (1977). Explorations using computer simulation to comprehend TAT measurement of motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., & Cartwright, D. (1964). Some neglected variables in contemporary conceptions of decision and performance. Psychological Reports, 14, 575–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., & Feather, N. T. (Eds.). (1966). A theory of achievement motivation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., Heyns, R. W., & Veroff, J. (1954). The effect of experimental arousal of the affiliation motive on thematic apperception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 405–410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. W., Lens, W., & O'Malley, P. M. (1976). Motivation and ability: Interactive psychological determinants of intellective performance, educational achievement, and each other. In Sewell, W. H., Hauser, R. M., & Featherman, D. L. (Eds.), Schooling and achievement in American society (pp. 29–60). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., & Litwin, G. H. (1960). Achievement motive and test anxiety conceived as motive to approach success and motive to avoid failure. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 52–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. W., & McClelland, D. C. (1948). The projective expression of needs: II. The effect of different intensities of the hunger drive in thematic apperception. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33, 643–658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., & Raynor, J. O. (1974). Motivation and achievement. Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., & Reitman, W. R. (1956). Performance as a function of motive strength and expectancy of goal attainment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53, 361–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. W., & Walker, E. L. (1956). The affiliation motive and perceptual sensitivity to faces. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53, 38–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and content. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 338–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aveling, F. (1926). The psychology of conation and will. British Journal of Psychology, 16, 339–353.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 5–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Boston: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Balagura, S. (1973). Hunger: A biopsychological analysis. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (1998). Life-span theory in developmental psychology. In Damon, W. & Lerner, R. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 1029–1143). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: A metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. American Psychologist, 55, 122–135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Band, E. B., & Weisz, J. R. (1988). How to feel better when it feels bad: Children's perspectives on coping with everyday stress. Developmental Psychology, 24, 246–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1971). Vicarious and self-reinforcement processes. In Glaser, R. (Ed.), The nature of reinforcement (pp. 228–278). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism. American Psychologist, 33, 344–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanisms in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1991). Self-regulation of motivation through anticipatory and self-reactive mechanisms. In Dienstbier, R. A. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 38. Perspectives on motivation (pp. 69–164). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1998). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Banse, R. (2001). Affective priming with liked and disliked persons: Prime visibility determines congruency and incongruency effects. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 501–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bär, R. (1998). Pornographie: Motive, Meinungen, sexuelle Phantasien und situative Kontexte [Pornography: Motives, opinions, sexual fantasies, and situational contexts]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Wuppertal, Germany.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of social interaction. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 93–130). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In Wyer, R. S. & Srull, T. (Eds), Handbook of social cognition: Vol. 1. Basic processes (2nd ed., pp. 1–41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In Wyer, R. S. (Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Barndollar, K. (1996). Automaticity in action: The unconscious as repository of chronic goals and motivation. In Gollwitzer, P. M. & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 457–481). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes, C. (1996). The automatic evaluation effect: Unconditional automatic attitude activation with a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 104–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype priming on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. L. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: On the automaticity of higher mental processes. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 925–945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bargh, J. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1994). Environmental control of goal-directed action: Automatic and strategic contingencies between situations and behavior. In Spaulding, W. D. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 41. Integrative views of motivation, cognition, and emotion (pp. 71–124). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Barker, R. G., & Gump, P. (Eds.). (1964). Big school, small school: High school size and student behavior. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Barnett, P. A., & Gotlib, I. C. (1988). Psychological functioning and depression: Distinguishing among antecedents, concomitants, and consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 97–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, K. C., & Morgan, G. A. (1995). Continuities and discontinuities in mastery motivation during infancy and toddlerhood: A conceptualization and review. In MacTurk, R. H. & Morgan, G. A. (Eds.), Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptualization and applications (pp. 57–93). Westport, CT: Ablex.Google Scholar
Barrett, K. C., Morgan, G. A., & Maslin-Cole, B. (1993). Three studies on the development of mastery motivation in infancy and toddlerhood. In Messer, D. (Ed.), Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and social processes (pp. 83–108). London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barron, K. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement goals and optimal motivation: Testing multiple goal models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 706–722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love. NeuroReport, 11(17), 3829–3834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. NeuroImage, 21(3), 1155–1166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartlett, E. W., & Smith, C. P. (1966). Childrearing practices, birth order and the development of achievement-related motives. Psychological Reports, 19, 1207–1216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baum, M. J. (1992). Neuroendocrinology of sexual behavior in the male. In Becker, J. B., Breedlove, S. M., & Crews, D. (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinology (pp. 97–130). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baumann, N., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Affect regulation and motive-incongruent achievement orientation: Antecedents of subjective well-being and symptom formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 781–799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2002). Intuition, affect and personality: Unconscious coherence judgments and self-regulation of negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1213–1223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Self-infiltration: Confusing assigned tasks as self-selected in memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 487–497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2004). How to resist temptation: The effects of external control versus autonomy support on the dynamics of self-regulation. Journal of Personality, 73, 443–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumann, N., Kuhl, J., & Kazén, (2004). Hemispheric activation and self infiltration: Testing a neuropsychological model of internalization. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 135–163.
Baumeister, R. F. (1984). Choking under pressure: Self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 610–620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Ego-depletion and the self's executive function. In Tesser, A. & Felson, R. B. (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on self and identity (pp. 9–33). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., & Showers, C. (1986). A review of paradoxical performance effects: Choking under pressure in sports and mental tests. European Journal of Social Psychology, 16, 361–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayer, U. C., Achtziger, A., Malzacher, J. T., Moskowitz, G. B., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (under review). Strategic automaticity by implementation intentions: Action initiation without conscious intent.
Baxter, M. G., & Murray, E. A. (2002). The amygdala and reward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(7), 563–573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayer, U. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). Mindset effects on information search in self-evaluation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 313–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayer, U. C., Jaudas, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2002, July). Do implementation intentions facilitate switching between tasks? Poster presented at the International Symposium on Executive Functions, Konstanz, Germany.Google Scholar
Beauregard, M., Levesque, J., & Bourgouin, P. (2001). Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 6993–7000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 295–307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275(5304), 1293–1295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression – Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Beckmann, J. (1984). Kognitive Dissonanz: Eine handlungstheoretische Perspektive [Cognitive dissonance: An action theory perspective]. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckmann, J. (1994). Rumination and deactivation of an intention. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 317–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckmann, J. (1996). Self-presentation and the Zeigarnik effect. In Gjesme, T. & Nygard, R. (Ed.), Advances in motivation (pp. 35–45). Oslo, Norway: Scandinavian University Press.Google Scholar
Beckmann, J., Bobka, K., Fehrenbach, H., Hellebrandt, M., & Rost, K. (2007). The perseverance of complete and incomplete intentions in memory: Zeigarnik effect or self serving recall?Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Beckmann, J., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Deliberative versus implemental states of mind: The issue of impartiality in predecisional and postdecisional information processing. Social Cognition, 5, 259–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckmann, J., & Kuhl, J. (1984). Altering information to gain action control: Functional aspects of human information processing in decision making. Journal of Research in Personality, 18, 224–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckmann, J., & Rolstad, K. (1997). Aktivierung, Selbstregulation und Leistung: Gibt es so etwas wie Übermotivation? [Activation, self-regulation, and performance: Is there such a thing as overmotivation?]Sportwissenschaft, 27, 23–37.Google Scholar
Beeman, M., Friedman, R. B., Grafman, J., Perez, E., Diamond, S., & Lindsay, M. B. (1994). Summation priming and coarse coding in the right hemisphere. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 26–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beit-Hallahmi, B. (1980). Achievement motivation and economic growth: A replication. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 210–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bem, D. J., & Allen, A. (1974). On predicting some of the people some of the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies in behavior. Psychological Review, 81, 506–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beneson, J., & Dweck, C. S. (1986). The development of trait explanation and self-evaluations in the academic and social domains. Child Development, 57, 1179–1189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, P., & Luckman, T. (1967). The social construction of reality. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Berkowitz, L. (1974). Some determinants of impulsive aggression: Role of mediated associations with reinforcements for aggression. Psychological Review, 81, 165–176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkowitz, L. (1990). On the formation and regulation of anger and aggression: A cognitive-neoassociationistic analysis. American Psychologist, 45, 494–503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. New York: MacGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Berkowitz, L. (1994). Is something missing? Some observations prompted by the cognitive-neoassociationist view of anger and emotional aggression. In Huesmann, L. R. (Ed.), Aggressive behaviour: Current perspectives (pp. 35–57). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Berkowitz, L., & LePage, A. (1967). Weapons as aggression-eliciting stimuli. Journal of Personalitiy and Social Psychology, 7, 202–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1963a). Complexity and incongruity variables as determinants of exploratory choice and evaluative ratings. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 17, 274–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1963b). Motivational problems raised by exploratory and epistemic behavior. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. V, pp. 284–364). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1965). Structure and direction in thinking. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1967). Arousal and reinforcement. In Levine, D. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1967 (pp. 1–110). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1973). The vicissitudes of aplopathematic and thelematoscopic pneumatology (or the hydrography of hedonism). In Berlyne, D. E. & Madsen, K. B. (Eds.), Pleasure, reward, preference (pp. 1–34). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (Ed.). (1974). Studies in the new experimental aesthetics. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E., & Crozier, J. B. (1971). Effects of complexity and prechoice stimulation on exploratory choice. Perception and Psychophysics, 10, 242–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernhardt, P. C., Dabbs, J. M., Jr., Fielden, J. A., & Lutter, C. D. (1998). Testosterone changes during vicarious experiences of winning and losing among fans at sporting events. Physiology & Behavior, 65, 59–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernoulli, D. (1738). Specimen theoriae novae de mensura sortis. Commentarii Academiae Scientiarum Imperialis Petropolitanae, 5, 175–192.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (1996). Food reward: Brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 20, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (2003). Comparing the emotional brains of humans and other animals. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 25–51). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience?Brain Research Review, 28, 309–369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (2003). Parsing reward. Trends in Neurosciences, 26(9), 507–513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beswick, G., & Mann, L. (1994). State orientation and procrastination. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Betancourt, H., & Blair, I. (1992). An attribution-emotion model of violence in conflict situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 343–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bexton, W. H., Heron, W., & Scott, T. H. (1954). Effects of decreased variation in the sensory environment. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 8, 70–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bieneck, A. (1991). Tätigkeitszentrierte Anreize des Skifahrens für Behinderte und Nichtbehinderte in Abhängigkeit vom Fähigkeitsstand [Activity-related incentives of skiing for disabled and nondisabled individuals as a function of ability level]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Bieri, P. (2001). Das Handwerk der Freiheit: Die Entdeckung des eigenen Willens [The craft of free time: Discovering one's own will]. Munich, Germany: Hanser.Google Scholar
Biernat, M. (1989). Motives and values to achieve: Different constructs with different effects. Journal of Personality, 57, 69–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billington, C. J., & Levine, A. S. (1992). Hypothalamic neuropeptide regulation of feeding and energy metabolism. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 2(6), 847–851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bindra, D. (1959). Motivation: A systematic reinterpretation.New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
Bindra, D. (1969). The interrelated mechanisms of reinforcement and motivation, and the nature of their influence on response. In Arnold, W. J. & Levine, D. (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1969 (pp. 1–38). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Bindra, D. (1974). A motivational view of learning, performance, and behavior modification. Psychological Review, 81, 199–213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bindra, D. (1978). How adaptive behavior is produced: A perceptual-motivational alternative to response-reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 41–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birenbaum, G. (1930). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs- und Affektpsychologie. VII. Das Vergessen einer Vornahme [Studies on the psychology of action and emotion. VIII. The forgetting of instructions]. Psychologische Forschung, 13, 1930, 218–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birney, R. C., Burdick, H., & Teevan, R. C. (1969). Fear of failure motivation.New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Bischof, N. (1975). A systems approach towards the functional connections of attachment and fear. Child Development, 46, 801–817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bischof, N. (1985). Das Rätsel Ödipus: Die biologischen Wurzeln des Urkonfliktes von Intimität und Autonomie [The enigma of Oedipus: Biological roots of the basic conflict between intimacy and autonomy]. Munich, Germany: Piper.Google Scholar
Bischof, N. (1993). Untersuchungen zur Systemanalyse der sozialen Motivation I: Die Regulation der sozialen Distanz – Von der Feldtheorie zur Systemtheorie [Social distance regulation – From “field” to “system”]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 201, 5–43.Google Scholar
Bischof, N. (1996). Untersuchungen zur Systemanalyse der sozialen Motivation IV: Die Spielarten des Lächelns und das Problem der motivationalen Sollwertanpassung [The varieties of smiling and the problem of motivational acclimatization]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 204, 1–40.Google Scholar
Bischoff, J. (2003). Lernmotivation, Flow-Erleben und Leistung in universitären Fremdsprachkursen [Motivation to learn, flow experience, and achievement in university second language courses]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Potsdam, Germany.Google Scholar
Bischof-Köhler, D. (1985). Zur Phylogenese menschlicher Motivation [On the phylogeny of human motivation]. In Eckensberger, L. H. & Lantermann, E.-D. (Eds.), Emotion und Reflexivität (pp. 3–47). Munich, Germany: Urban & Schwarzenberg.Google Scholar
Bitterman, M. E. (1975). The comparative analysis of learning. Science, 188, 699–709.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Björkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., & Kaukiainen, A. (1992). Do girls manipulate and boys fight? Developmental trends in regard to direct and indirect aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 18, 117–127.3.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Björkqvist, K., & Niemelä, P. (Eds.). (1992). Of mice and women: Aspects of female aggression. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Blankenship, V. (1984). Computer anxiety and self-concept of ability. In Schwarzer, R. (Ed.), The self in anxiety, stress, and depression (pp. 151–158). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Blankenship, V. (1987). A computer-based measure of resultant achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 361–372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the organization of behavior. In Collins, W. A. (Ed.), Development of cognition, affect, and social relations: The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 39–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Blodgett, H. C. (1929). The effect of the introduction of reward upon the maze performance of rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 113–134.Google Scholar
Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. PNAS, 98(20), 11818–11823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blossfeld, H. P. (1987). Labor market entry and the sexual segregation of careers in the Federal Republic of Germany. American Journal of Sociology, 93, 89–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blossfeld, H. P. (1988). Sensible Phasen im Bildungsverlauf – Eine Längsschnittanalyse über die Prägung von Bildungskarrieren durch den gesellschaftlichen Wandel [Critical phases in educational careers – A longitudinal study on the influence of societal changes on educational careers]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 34, 45–63.Google Scholar
Blossfeld, H. P., & Mayer, K. U. (1988). Labor market segmentation in the Federal Republic of Germany: An empirical study of segmentation theories from a life course perspective. European Sociological Review, 4, 123–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bludau, H. F. (1976). Mobilitätstendenzen im Gefangenen-Dilemma-Spiel [Mobility tendencies in the prisoner's dilemma game]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Blunt, A., & Pychyl, T. A. (1998). Volitional action and inaction in the lives of undergraduate students: State orientation, procrastination and proneness to boredom. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 837–846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boekaerts, M. (2003). Towards a model that integrates motivation, affect and learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, Monograph Series II, Part 2 (Development and Motivation: Joint Perspectives), 173–189.Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1965). Readiness to eat: Effects of age, sex and weight loss. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 60, 88–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolles, R. C. (1967). Theory of motivation.New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1972). Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning. Psychological Review, 79, 394–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1974). Cognition and motivation: Some historical trends. In Weiner, B. (Ed.), Cognitive views of human motivation (pp. 1–20). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1975). Theory of motivation (2nd ed.). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Bolte, A., Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Emotion and intuition: Effects of positive and negative mood on implicit judgments of semantic coherence. Psychological Science, 14, 416–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonsfield, W. A. (1953). The occurrence of clustering in the recall of randomly arranged associates. Journal of General Psychology, 49, 229–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1929). A history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, F. (1993). NEO-Fünf-Faktoren-Inventar (NEO-FFI) nach Costa und McCrae: Handanweisung [The Neo Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) by Costa and McCrae: Manual]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Bornstein, R. F. (2002). A process dissociation approach to objective-projective test score interrelationships. Journal of Personality Assessment, 78, 47–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosson, J. K., Swann, W. B., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant revisited?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 631–643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouton, M. E., & Fanselow, M. S. (1997). Learning, motivation and cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Bower, G. H. (1981). Emotional mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowers, K. S. (1973). Situationism in psychology: An analysis and a critique. Psychological Review, 80, 307–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowi, U. (1990). Der Einfluss von Motiven auf Zielsetzung und Zielrealisation [The influence of motives on goal setting and goal implementation]. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child's tie to his mother. Interactional Journal of Psychoanalysis, 39, 350–373.Google ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. In Khan, M. M. (Ed.), Attachment 1. London: Hogarth.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd rev. ed.). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boyatzis, R. E. (1972). A two factor theory of affiliation motivation.. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Boyatzis, R. E. (1973). Affiliation motivation. In McClelland, D. C. & Steele, R. S. (Eds.), Human motivation: A book of readings (pp. 253–276). Morristown, NJ: General Learning.Google Scholar
Boyce, R. (1976). In the shadow of Darwin. In Green, R. G. & Neil, E. C. O' (Eds.), Perspectives in aggression (pp. 11–35). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Brackhane, R. (1976). Bezugssysteme im Leistungsverhalten [Reference systems in achievement behavior]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Münster, Germany.Google Scholar
Bradley, G. W. (1978). Self-serving biases in the attribution process: A reexamination of the fact or fiction question. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 56–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandtstädter, J. (1984). Personal and social control over development: Some implications of an action perspective in life-span developmental psychology. In Baltes, P. B. & Brim, O. G. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 6) (pp. 1–32). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Brandtstädter, J. (1986). Personale Entwicklungskontrolle und entwicklungsregulatives Handeln: Überlegungen und Befunde zu einem vernachlässigten Forschungsthema [Personal control and regulative action in development: Thoughts and findings on an underrated issue of research]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 18, 316–334.Google Scholar
Brandtstädter, J. (1998). Action perspectives on human development. Handbook of Child Psychology, 1, 807–863.Google Scholar
Brandtstädter, J. (2001). Entwicklung, Intentionalität, Handeln [Development, intentionality, action]. Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Brandtstädter, J., & Greve, W. (1994). The aging self: Stabilizing and protective processes. Developmental Review, 14, 52–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandtstädter, J., & Lerner, R. (Eds.). (1999). Action and self development: Theory and research through the life span. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Brandtstädter, J., & Renner, G. (1990). Tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal adjustment: Explication and age-related analysis of assimilative and accommodative strategies of coping. Psychology and Aging 5, 58–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandtstädter, J., & Rothermund, K. (2002). The life-course dynamics of goal pursuit and goal adjustment: A two process framework. Developmental Review, 22, 117–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandtstädter, J., & Rothermund, K. (2003). Intentionality and time in human development and aging: Compensation and goal adjustment in changing developmental contexts. In Staudinger, U. M. & Lindenberger, U. (Eds.), Understanding human development: Dialogues with lifespan psychology (pp. 105–124). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandtstädter, J., Wentura, D., & Rothermund, K. (1999). Intentional self-development through adulthood and later life: Tenacious pursuit and flexible adjustment of goals. In Brandstädter, J. & Lerner, R. (Eds.), Action and self development: Theory and research through the life span (pp. 373–400). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Brandstätter, V., & Frank, E. (2002). Effects of deliberative and implemental mindsets on persistence in goal-directed behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1366–1378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946–960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brauckmann, L. (1976). Erstellung und Erprobung eines Lehrerverhaltenstrainings zur Veränderung der motivanregenden Bedingungen des Unterrichts[Development and testing of a teacher training program targeting the motivating conditions of instruction]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Braun, W., & Proebsting, H. (1986). Heiratstafeln verwitweter Deutscher 1979/82 und geschiedener Deutscher 1980/83 [Marriage tables of widowed, 1979/82, and divorced, 1980/83, Germans]. Wirtschaft und Statistik, 107–112.Google Scholar
Breckler, S. J., & Greenwald, A. G. (1986). Motivational facets of the self. In Sorrentino, R. M. & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (pp. 145–164). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Brehm, J. W. (1956). Post decision changes in the desirability of alternatives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 384–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehm, J. W., & Cohen, A. R. (1962). Explorations in cognitive dissonance. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breland, K., & Breland, M. (1961). The misbehavior of animals. American Psychologist, 16, 681–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In Srull, T. K. & Wyer, R. S. Jr. (Eds.), A dual process model of impression formation (pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brewin, C. R., Dalgleish, T., & Joseph, S. (1996). A dual representation theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Review, 103, 670–686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brock, D. B., Guralnick, J. M., & Brody, J. A. (1990). Demography and epidemiology of aging in the United States: Handbook of the biology of aging (Vol. 3, pp. 3–23). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1988). The ecology of developmental processes. In Damon, W. & Lerner, M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 993–1028). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brown, J. F. (1933). Über die dynamischen Eigenschaften der Realitäts- und Irrealitätsschichten [On the dynamic properties of the levels of reality and unreality]. Psychologische Forschung, 18, 1–26.Google Scholar
Brown, J. S. (1948). Gradients of approach and avoidance responses and their relation to model of motivation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 41, 450–465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J. S. (1953). Problems presented by the concept of acquired drives. In Brown, J. S. & Jacobs, A. (Eds.), Current theory and research in motivation: A symposium (pp. 1–21). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Brown, J. S. (1961). The motivation of behavior.New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, P. L., & Jenkins, H. M. (1968). Auto-shaping of the pigeon's key-peck. Journal of Analysis of Behavior, 11, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunstein, J. C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1061–1070.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C. (1995). Motivation nach Misserfolg: Zur Bedeutung von Commitment und Substitution [Motivation following failure: The role of commitment and substitution]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Persönliche Ziele und subjektives Wohlbefinden bei älteren Menschen [Personal goals and subjective well-being among older adults]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 20, 58–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C. (2001). Persönliche Ziele und Handlungs- versus Lageorientierung: Wer bindet sich an realistische und bedürfniskongruente Ziele? [Personal goals and action versus state orientation: Who builds a commitment to realistic and need-congruent goals?]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 22, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C. (2003). Implizite Motive und motivationale Selbstbilder: Zwei Prädiktoren mit unterschiedlicher Gültigkeit [Implicit motives and motivational self-concepts: Two predictors with differing validity]. In Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. & Rheinberg, F. (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept (Tests and Trends Vol. 2, pp. 59–88). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., Ganserer, J., Maier, H. & Heckhausen, H. (1991). Persönliche Anliegen in Alltagssituationen [Personal concerns in everyday situations]. Memorandum No. 82.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1996). Effects of failure on subsequent performance: The importance of self-defining goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 395–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunstein, J. C., & Hoyer, S. (2002). Implizites versus explizites Leistungsstreben: Befunde zur Unabhängigkeit zweier Motivationssysteme [Implicit versus explicit achievement strivings: Empirical evidence of the independence of two motivational systems]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 16, 51–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., Lautenschlager, U., Nawroth, B., Pöhlmann, K., & Schultheiss, O. C. (1995). Persönliche Anliegen, soziale Motive und emotionales Wohlbefinden [Personal goals, social motives, and emotional well-being]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 16, 1–10.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (1996). Persönliche Ziele: Ein Überblick zum Stand der Forschung [Personal goals: A state-of-the-art review]. Psychologische Rundschau, 47, 146–160.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (2001). Das Streben nach persönlichen Zielen: Emotionales Wohlbefinden und proaktive Entwicklung über die Lebensspanne [Striving for personal goals: Emotional well-being and proactive development across the lifespan]. In Jüttemann, G. & Thomae, H. (Eds.), Persönlichkeit und Entwicklung (pp. 155–188). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (2005). Implicit and self-attributed motives to achieve: Two separate but interacting needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 205–222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunstein, J. C., Maier, G. W., & Schultheiss, O. C. (1999). Motivation und Persönlichkeit: Von der Analyse von Teilsystemen zur Analyse ihrer Interaktionen [Motivation and personality: From the analysis of subsystems to the analyses of their interactions]. In Jerusalem, M. & Pekrun, R. (Eds.), Emotion, Motivation und Leistung (pp. 147–167). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Olbrich, E. (1985). Personal helplessness and action control: An analysis of achievement-related cognitions, self-assessments, and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1540–1551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Schmitt, C. H. (2003). Prüfung der konvergenten, diskriminanten und prädiktiven Validität von Leistungsmotiv-IATs, -TATs und -Fragebögen [Testing the convergent, discriminant, and predictive validity of the achievement motive IAT, TAT, and questionnaires] (DFG Report). Potsdam, Germany: University of Potsdam.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Schmitt, C. H. (2004). Assessing individual differences in achievement motivation with the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 536–555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 494–508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Maier, G. W. (1999). The pursuit of personal goals: A motivational approach to well-being and life adjustment. In Brandtstädter, J. & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Action and self-development: Theory and research through the life span (pp. 169–196). London: Sage.Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1952). The conceptual frame work of psychology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and representative design of psychological experiments. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Bryk, A., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models for social and behavioral research: Applications and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Buber, M. (1979). I and thou. New York: Scribner's.Google Scholar
Buck, R. (1985). Prime theory: An integrated view of motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 389–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bühler, C. (1933). Der menschliche Lebenslauf als psychologisches Problem [The human life course as a topic of psychology]. Leipzig, Germany: Hirzel.Google Scholar
Bühler, C., Hetzer, H., & Mabel, F. (1928). Die Affektwirksamkeit von Fremdheitseindrücken im ersten Lebensjahr [The emotional effects of unfamiliarity in the first year of life]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 107, 30–40.Google Scholar
Bühler, C., & Marschak, M. (1969). Grundtendenzen des menschlichen Lebens [Basic tendencies of human life]. In Bühler, C. & Massarik, F. (Eds.), Lebenslauf und Lebensziele (pp. 78–88). Stuttgart, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Bühler, K. (1922). Die geistige Entwicklung des Kindes [The mental development of the child] (3rd ed.). Jena, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Bühler, K.-E., & Heim, G. (2002). Psychisches Trauma und fixe Ideen [Psychic trauma and fixed ideas]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 50, 394–408.Google Scholar
Bullock, M., & Lütkenhaus, P. (1988). The development of volitional behavior in the toddler years. Child Development, 59, 664–674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bullock, M., & Lütkenhaus, P. (1991). Who am I? Self understanding in toddlers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 217–238.Google Scholar
Buss, A. H. (1980). Self-consciousness and social anxiety. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1987). Selection, evocation and manipulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1214–1221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary personality psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 42, 459–491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, D. M. (2001). Human nature and culture: An evolutionary psychological perspective. Journal of Personality, 69, 955–978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204–232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, H. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Butler, R. (1993). Effects of task- and ego-achievement goals on information-seeking during task engagement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 18–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, R. (1999). Information seeking and achievement motivation in middle childhood and adolescence: The role of conceptions of ability. Developmental Psychology, 35, 146–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, R. (2000). What learners want to know: The role of achievement goals in shaping information seeking, learning, and interest. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 162–194). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Butterfield, E. C. (1964). The interruption of tasks: Methodological, factual and theoretical issues. Psychological Bulletin, 62, 309–322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butz, M. V., & Hoffmann, J. (2002). Anticipations control behavior: Animal behavior in an anticipatory learning classifier system. Adaptive Behavior, 10, 75–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, D. (1961a). Anxiety and the experimental arousal of affiliation need. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 660–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, D. (1961b). The repression-sensitization scale: Rationale, reliability, and validity. Journal of Personality, 29, 334–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, D. (1962). Response to attitude similarity-dissimilarity as a function of affiliation need. Journal of Personality, 30, 164–177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, D., McDonald, R. D., & Mikawa, J. (1963). Approach and avoidance affiliation motives. Journal of Personality, 31, 21–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabanac, M. (1971). Physiological role of pleasure. Science, 173(2), 1103–1107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cahill, L. (2000). Modulation of long-term memory in humans by emotional arousal: Adrenergic activation and the amygdala. In Aggleton, J. P. (Ed.), The amygdala: A functional analysis (pp. 425–446). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cahill, L., Prins, B., Weber, M., & McGaugh, J. L. (1994). Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature, 371, 702–704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calder, B., & Staw, B. M. (1975). The interaction of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Some methodological notes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 76–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, J., Banko, K. M., & Pierce, W. D. (2001). Pervasive negative effects of rewards on intrinsic motivation: The myth continues. The Behavior Analyst, 24, 1–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, B. A., & Sheffield, F. D. (1953). Relation of random activity to food deprivation. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 46, 320–322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, J. P., Dunnette, M. D., Lawler, E. E., & Weick, K. E. (1970). Managerial behavior performance and effectiveness. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Cannon, W. B., & Washburn, A. L. (1912). An explanation of hunger. American Journal of Physiology, 29, 441–454.Google Scholar
Canon, L. K. (1964). Self-confidence and selective exposure to information. In Festinger, L. (Ed.), Conflict, decision, and dissonance (pp. 83–95). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Cantor, N., & Fleeson, W. W. (1991). Life tasks and self-regulatory processes. In Maehr, M. L. & Pintrich, P. R. (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 7, pp. 327–269). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Cantor, N., Mischel, W., & Schwartz, J. C. (1982). A prototype analysis of psychological situations. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 45–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cantor, N., Norem, J. K., Niedenthal, M., & Brower, A. (1987). Life tasks, self-concept ideals, and cognitive strategies in a life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1178–1191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cantor, N., & Zirkel, S. (1990). Personality, cognition, and purposive behavior. In Pervin, L. A. (Ed.), Handbook of personality research: Theory and research (pp. 135–164). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cardinal, R. N., Parkinson, J. A., Hall, J., & Everitt, B. J. (2002). Emotion and motivation: The role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 321–352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, E. R. (1956). Attitude change through modification of attitude structure. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 256–261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, N. R. (1994). Physiology of behavior (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Caron, A. J., & Wallach, M. A. (1957). Recall of interrupted tasks under stress: A phenomenon of memory or learning?Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55, 372–381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caron, A. J., & Wallach, M. A. (1959). Personality determinants of repressive and obsessive reactions to failure stress. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 236–245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, S. J., Dabbs, J. M., & Carr, G. (1975). Mother-infant attachment: The importance of the mother's visual field. Child Development, 46(2), 331–338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, L., Voisey, J., & Daal, A. (2004). Mouse models of obesity. Clinics in Dermatology, 22(4), 345–349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 779–818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cartwright, D. (1942). The effect of interruption, completion and failure upon the attractiveness of activity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartwright, D. (1959). A field theory conception of power. In Cartwright, D. (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 183–220). Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Cartwright, D. (1965). Influence, leadership, and control. In March, J. G. (Ed.), Handbook of organizations (pp. 1–47). Chicago: Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Cartwright, D., & Festinger, L. (1943). A quantitative theory of decision. Psychological Review, 50, 595–621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S., Lawrence, J. W., & Scheier, M. E. (1996). A control-process perspective on the origins of affect. In Martin, L. L. & Tesser, A. (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 11–52). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A. (1998). Personality development across the life course. Handbook of Child Psychology, 3(5), 311–388.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1950). Personality: A systematic, theoretical, and factual study. New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1957). Personality and motiva tion: Structure and measurement. Yonkers, NY: World Book.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1958). Extracting the correct number of factors in factor analysis. Educational Psychological Measurement, 18, 791–838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1965). The scientific analysis of personality.Baltimore: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1974). Handbook of modern personality theory.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Cervone, D. (2004). The architecture of personality. Psychological Review, 111, 183–204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charlesworth, W. R. (1969). The role of surprise in cognitive development. In Elkind, D. & Flavell, J. H. (Eds.), Studies in cognitive development (pp. 257–314). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chasteen, A. L., Park, D., & Schwarz, N. (2001). Implementation intentions and facilitation of prospective memory. Psychological Science, 12, 457–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 330–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious behavioral confirmation processes: The self-fulfilling consequences of automatic stereotype activation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 541–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, P. W., & Novick, L. R. (1990a). Where is the bias in causal attribution? In Gilholey, K. J., Kayne, M. T. K., Logie, R. H., & Erdos, G. (Eds.), Lines of thinking (pp. 181–197). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cheng, P. W., & Novick, L. R. (1990b). A probabilistic contrast model of causal induction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 545–567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chipperfield, J. G., Pery, R. P., & Menec, A. (1999). Primary and secondary control-enhancing strategies: Implications for health in later life. Journal of Aging and Health, 11, 517–539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, K. (1999). Hypophysen-Gonaden-Achse (Mann) [Hypophyse-gonad axis (male)]. In Kirschbaum, C. & Hellhammer, D. (Eds.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Biologische Psychologie, Band 3: Psychoendokrinologie und Psychoimmunologie (pp. 141–222). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Chusmir, L. H. (1984). Motivational need pattern for police officers. Journal of Police Science & Administration, 12, 141–145.Google Scholar
Claessens, D. (1968). Instinkt, Psyche, Geltung: Bestimmungsfaktoren menschlichen Verhaltens. Eine soziologische Anthropologie [Instinct, psyche, status: Determining factors in human behavior. A sociological anthropology]. Köln (Cologne), Germany: Westdeutscher Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, P., & James, J. (1967). The effects of situation, attitude intensity and personality on information-seeking. Sociometry, 30, 235–245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, R. A. (1952). The projective measurement of experimental induced levels of sexual motivation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 391–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, R. A., & Sensibar, M. R. (1955). The relationship between symbolic and manifest projections of sexuality with some incidental correlates. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 50, 327–334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1973). Interactions between mothers and their young children: Characteristics and consequences. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(153), 6–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke-Stewart, K. A., VanderStoep, L. P., & Killian, P. (1979). Analysis and replication of mother-child relations at two years of age. Child Development, 50(3), 777–793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clavadetscher, C. (2003). Motivation ehrenamtlicher Arbeit im Verein Mahogany Hall, Bern [Motivation for voluntary work in the Mahogany Hall Society, Berne, Switzerland]. Unpublished final thesis, Berne University of Applied Sciences, Switzerland.Google Scholar
Clemente, C. D., & Chase, M. H. (1973). Neurological substrates of aggressive behavior. Annual Review of Physiology, 35, 329–356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coats, E. J., Janoff-Bulman, R., & Alpert, N. (1996). Approach versus avoidance goals: Differences in self-evaluation and well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1057–1067.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cofer, C. N., & Appley, M. H. (1964). Motivation: Theory and research. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Collins, C. J., Hanges, P. J., & Locke, E. A. (2004). The relation of achievement motivation to entrepreneurial behavior: A meta-analysis. Human Performance, 17, 95–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collis, G. M., & Schaffer, H. R. (1975). Synchronization of visual attention in mother-infant pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16, 315–320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compas, B. E. (1987). Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 393–403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compas, B. E., & Worsham, N. L. (1991, April). When mom or dad has cancer: Developmental differences in children's coping with family stress. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development meeting, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Compas, B. E., Worsham, N. L., Ey, L., & Howell, B. (1996). When mom or dad has cancer II: Coping, cognitive appraisals, and psychological distress in children of cancer patients. Health Psychology, 15, 167–175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Constantian, C. A. (1981). Attitudes, beliefs, and behavior in regard to spending time alone. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Parker, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261–288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, W. H. (1983). An achievement motivation nomological network. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 841–861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coopersmith, S. (1960). Self-esteem and need achievement as determinants of selective recall and repetition. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 310–317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corr, P. J., Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (1997). Personality, punishment, and procedural learning: A test of J. A. Gray's anxiety theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(2), 337–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 163–228). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1994). Origins of domain-specificity: The evolution of functional organization. In Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 85–116). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). From catalogue to classification: Murray's needs and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 258–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Covington, M. V. (1992). Making the grade: A self-worth perspective on motivation and school reform. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Covington, M. V., & Omelich, C. L. (1979). Are arousal attributions causal? A path analysis of the cognitive model of achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1487–1504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Covington, M. V., & Omelich, C. L. (1991). Need achievement revisited: Verification of Atkinson's original 2×2 model. In Spielberger, C. D., Sarason, I. G., Kulcsár, Z., & Heck, G. L. (Eds.), Stress and emotion: Anxiety, anger, and curiosity (Vol. 14, pp. 85–105). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Covington, M. V., & Roberts, B. W. (1994). Self-worth and college achievement: Motivational and personality correlates. In Pintrich, P. R., Brown, D. R., & Weinstein, C. E. (Eds.), Student motivation, cognition, and learning (pp. 157–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cowlishaw, G., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (1991). Dominance, rank, and mating success in male primates. Animal Behavior, 41, 1045–1056.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coyne, J. C., & Gotlib, I. C. H. (1983). The role of cognitions in depression: A critical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 472–505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, W. (1918). Appetites and aversions as constituents of instincts. Biological Bulletin of Woods Hole, 34, 91–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., Moroz, T. M., Moscovitch, M., Stuss, D. T., Winocur, G., Tulving, E., & Kapur, S. (1999). In search of the self: A positron emission tomography study. Psychological Science, 10, 26–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crespi, L. P. (1942). Quantitative variation of incentive and performance in the white rat. American Journal of Psychology, 55, 467–517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crespi, L. P. (1944). Amount of reinforcement and level of performance. Psychological Review, 51, 341–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cronbach, L. J. (1990). Essentials of psychological testing (5th ed.). New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Crozier, W. R. (1979). Shyness as anxious self-preoccupation. Psychological Reports, 44, 959–962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention. (1999). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. Gainesville: NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, University of Florida.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1991). Das Flow-Erlebnis und seine Bedeutung für die Psychologie des Menschen [The flow experience and its significance for human psychology]. In Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.), Die außergewöhnliche Erfahrung im Alltag. Die Psychologie des Flow-Erlebens (pp. 28–49). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997a). Dem Sinn des Lebens eine Zukunft geben [The evolving self: A psychology for the third millennium]. Stuttgart, Germany: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997b). Finding flow. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Cskiszentmihalyi, M., & Beattie, O. (1979). Life themes: A theoretical and empirical exploration of their origins and effects. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 19, 45–63.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R., & Prescott, S. (1977). The ecology of adolescence acivity and experience. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 6, 281–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 815–822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cube v., F. (2003). Lust auf Leistung [Appetite for achievement]. Munich, Germany: Piper.Google Scholar
Cunningham, J. D., & Kelley, H. H. (1975). Causal attributions for personal events of varying magnitudes. Journal of Personality, 43, 74–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dabbs, J. M., Frady, R. L., Carr, T. S., & Besch, N. F. (1987). Saliva testosterone and criminal violence in young adult prison inmates. Psychosomatic Medicine, 49, 174–182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dabbs, J. M., & Hargrove, M. F. (1997). Age, testosterone, and behavior among female prison inmates. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 477–480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, R. A. (1957). The concept of power. Behavioral Sciences, 2, 201–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1983). Sex, evolution, and behavior (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. London: Papermac.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (2000). Ich fühle also bin ich: Die Entschlüsselung des Bewusstseins [I feel, therefore I am: Decoding consciousness]. Munich, Germany: List.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. C. (1991). Somatic markers and the guidance of behavior: Theory and preliminary testing (pp. 217–229). In Levin, H. S., Eisenberg, H. M., & Benton, A. L. (Eds.), Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp. 230–255). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Daniels, D., & Plomin, R. (1985). Origins of individual differences in infant shyness. Developmental Psychology, 21, 118–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dannefer, D. (1989). Human action and its place in theories of aging. Journal of Aging and Health, 3, 1–20.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). Origin of species by means of natural selection. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, R. J. (2000). Affective style, psychopathology, and resilience: Brain mechanisms and plasticity. American Psychologist, 55(11), 1196–1214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. J. (2001). Toward a biology of personality and emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 935, 191–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, R. J., Kalin, N. H., & Shelton, S. E. (1993). Lateralized response to diazepam predicts temperamental style in rhesus monkeys. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 1106–1110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dawson, M. E., & Schell, A. M. (1982). Electrodermal responses to attended and nonattended significant stimuli during dichotic listening. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 315–324.Google ScholarPubMed
Araujo, I. E., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., & Hobden, P. (2003). Representation of umami taste in the human brain. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90(1), 313–319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCasper, A. J., & Carstens, A. A. (1981). Contingencies of stimulation: Effects of learning and emotion in neonates [Special issue: Variability in infancy]. Infant Behavior and Development, 4(1), 19–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
deCharms, R. (1976). Enhancing motivation: Change in the classroom. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
deCharms, R. (1979). Motivation in der Klasse [Motivation in the classroom]. Munich, Germany: MVG.Google Scholar
deCharms, R., & Moeller, G. H. (1962). Values expressed in American children's readers: 1800–1950. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64, 136–142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
deCharms, R., Morrison, H. W., Reitman, W., & McClelland, D. C. (1955). Behavioral correlates of directly and indirectly measured achievement motivation. In McClelland, D. C. (Ed.), Studies in motivation (pp. 414–423). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1980). The empirical exploration of intrinsic motivational processes. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 39–80). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dreu, C. K. W., Carnevale, P. J. D., Emans, B. J. M., & Vliert, E. (1994). Effects of gain-loss frames in negotiation: Loss aversion, mismatching, and frame adoption. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 60, 90–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deese, J., & Carpenter, J. A. (1951). Drive level and reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42, 236–238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fave, Delle A., & Bassi, M. (2000). The quality of experience in adolescents' daily lives: Developmental perspectives. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 126, 347–367.Google Scholar
Delville, Y., DeVries, G. J., & Ferris, C. F. (2000). Neural connections of the anterior hypothalamus and agonistic behavior in golden hamsters. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 55, 53–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dembo, T. (1931). Der Ärger als dynamisches Problem [Anger as a dynamic problem]. Psychologische Forschung, 15, 1–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dempster, F. N. (1985). Proactive interference in sentence recall: Topic-similarity effects and individual differences. Memory and Cognition, 13, 81–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. A., Luciana, M., Arbisi, P., Collins, P., & Leon, A. (1994). Dopamine and the structure of personality: Relation of agonist-induced dopamine activity to positive emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(3), 485–498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. A., & Morrone-Strupinsky, J. V. (2005). A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: Implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(3), 313–350; discussion 350–395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1991). The adaptive base of the neural hierarchy: Elementary motivational controls on network function. In Dienstbier, R. A. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 289–342). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Devine, P. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devinsky, O. (2000). Right cerebral hemisphere dominance for a sense of corporeal and emotional self. Epilepsy & Behavior, 1, 60–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dibbelt, S. (1997). Wechseln und Beibehalten von Zielen als Subfunktionen der Handlungskontrolle [Changing and keeping goals as subfunctions of action control]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Osnabrück, Germany.Google Scholar
Dickhäuser, O., & Galfe, E. (2004). Besser als …, schlechter als … – Leistungsbezogene Vergleichsprozesse in der Grundschule [Better …, worse … – Achievement-related comparison processes in elementary school]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 36, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickhäuser, O., & Rheinberg, F. (2003). Bezugsnormorientierung: Erfassung, Probleme, Perspektiven [Reference norm orientations: Measurement, problems, perspectives]. In Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. & Rheinberg, F. (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept (pp. 41–55). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Dickhäuser, O., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2000). Geschlechtsunterschiede im Lern- und Leistungsverhalten am Computer: Ein theoretischer Rahmen [Gender differences in learning and achievement behavior on the computer: A theoretical framework]. In Försterling, F., Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Silny, L. (Eds.), Kognitive und emotionale Aspekte der Motivation (pp. 53–76). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Dickhäuser, O., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2002). Erlernte Hilflosigkeit am Computer? Geschlechtsunterschiede in computerspezifischen Attributionen [Learned helplessness at the computer? Gender differences in computer-specific attributions]. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 49, 44–55.Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. (1997). Bolles's psychological syllogism. In Bouton, M. E. & Fanselow, M. S. (Eds.), Learning, motivation and cognition (pp. 345–367). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Dickinson, A., & Shanks, D. (1995). Instrumental action and causal representation. In Sperber, D., Premack, D., & Premack, A. James (Eds.), A causal cognition (pp. 3–25). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Diefendorff, J. M., Hall, R. J., Lord, R. G., & Strean, M. L. (2000). Action-state orientation: Construct validity of a revised measure and its relationship to work-related variables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 250–263.Google ScholarPubMed
Diener, C. I., & Dweck, C. S. (1978). An analysis of learned helplessness: Continuous changes in performance, strategy, and achievement cognitions following failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 451–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Implications for mental and physical health. Psychological Review, 96, 84–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dilthey, W. (1894). Ideen über eine beschreibende und zergliedernde Psychologie [Ideas on a descriptive and analytical psychology]. In Sitzungsberichte der Königlichen Preußischen Akademie der Wissenschaften zu Berlin (Phil. hist. Classe) (pp. 1309–1407). Berlin, Germany: LIII.Google Scholar
DiVitto, B., & McArthur, L. Z. (1978). Developmental differences in the use of distinctiveness, consensus, and consistency information for making causal attributions. Developmental Psychology, 14, 474–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dix, T., Ruble, D. N., & Zambarano, R, (1989). Mothers' implicit theories of discipline: Child effects, parent effects, and the attribution process. Child Development, 60, 1373–1391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A. (1993). Social-cognitive mechanism in the development of conduct disorder and depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 559–584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A., & Colie, J. D. (1987). Social information processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children's peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1146–1158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollard, J., Doob, L., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, H. O., & Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustration and aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality and psychotherapy: An analysis in terms of learning, thinking, and culture. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Domjan, M., Blesbois, E., & Williams, J. (1998). The adaptive significance of sexual conditioning: Pavlovian control of sperm release. Psychological Science, 9, 411–415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donders, F. C. (1862). Die Schnelligkeit psychischer Prozesse [The speed of mental processes]. Archiv für Anatomie und Physiologie, 657–681.Google Scholar
Donley, R. E., & Winter, D. G. (1970). Measuring the motives of public officials at a distance: An exploratory study of American presidents. Behavioral Sciences, 15, 227–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, M. (1986). How institutions think. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.Google Scholar
Droege, K. L., & Stipek, D. J. (1993). Children's use of dispositions to predict classmates' behavior. Developmental Psychology, 29, 646–654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, E. (1932). The relationship between muscular tension and quality of performance. American Journal of Psychology, 44, 535–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, E. (1934). Emotion: An example of the need for reorientation in psychology. Psychological Review, 41, 184–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, E. (1941). An explanation of “emotional” phenomena without the use of the concept “emotion.” Journal of General Psychology, 25, 283–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, E. (1957). The psychological significance of the concept of “arousal” or “activation.” Psychological Review, 64, 265–275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duffy, E. (1962). Activation and behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Düker, H. (1931). Psychologische Unter suchungen über freie und zwangsläufige Arbeitsweise: Experimentelle Beiträge zur Willens- und Arbeitspsychologie [Psy chological studies of free and inevitable work: Experimental contributions on the psychology of volition and work]. Leipzig, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
Düker, H. (1975). Untersuchungen über die Ausbildung des Wollens [Studies on the development of volition]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (1975). The role of expectations and attributions in the alleviation of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 674–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (2002). The development of ability conceptions. In Wigfield, A. & Eccles, J. S. (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 57–88). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (2003). Clarifying achievement goals and their impact. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 541–553.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S., & Elliott, E. S. (1983). Achievement motivation. In Hetherington, E. M. (Ed.), Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 643–691). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. I. (1973). Learned helplessness and reinforcement responsibility in children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 25, 109–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, F. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, C. S., & Repucci, N. D. (1973). Learned helplessness and reinforcement responsibility in children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 25, 109–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior. American Psychologist, 54, 408–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eaves, G., & Rush, A. J. (1984). Cognitive patterns in symptomatic and remitted unipolar major depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 31–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ebbinghaus, H. (1902). Abriß der Psychologie [Summary of psychology]. Leipzig, Germany: Veit.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (1984). Sex differences in achievement patterns. In Sonderegger, T. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium of Motivation: Vol. 32 (pp. 97–132). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (1987). Gender roles and women's achievement-related decisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 135–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (2005). Subjective task value and the Eccles et al. model of achievement related choices. In Elliot, A. J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 105–121). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S., Adler, T. F., & Meece, J. L. (1984). Sex differences in achievement: A test of alternate theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(1), 26–43.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S., Barber, B., & Jozefowicz, D. (1999). Linking gender to educational, occupational and recreational choices: Applying the Eccles model of achievement related choices. In Swann, W. B. & Langlois, J. H. (Eds.), Sexism and stereotypes in modern society (pp. 153–192). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1995). In the mind of the actor: The structure of adolescents' achievement task values and expectancy-related beliefs. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(3), 215–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 109–132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Harold, R., & Blumenfeld, P. B. (1993). Age and gender differences in children's self- and task perceptions during elementary school. Child Development, 64, 830–847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (Volume Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3, 5th ed., pp. 1017–1095). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Eckhardt, G. (1968). Die entwicklungspsychologische Abhängigkeit der Konfliktreaktion vom Grad der Misserfolgswahrscheinlichkeit [A developmental perspective the dependence of conflict responses on the likelihood of failure]. Unpublished pre-Diplom thesis, University of Münster, Germany.
Edwards, W. (1954). The theory of decision-making. Psychological Bulletin, 51, 380–417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, W. (1962). Utility, subjective probability, their interaction, and variance preferences. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 6, 42–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S. C. (2002). Predictive validity of an Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1441–1455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrlich, D., Guttmann, I., Schönbach, P., & Mills, J. (1957). Postdecision exposure to relevant information. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 98–102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1973). Der vorprogrammierte Mensch [The preprogrammed human]. Vienna, Austria: Molden.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1975). Krieg und Frieden aus der Sicht der Verhaltensforschung [The biology of peace and war: Men, animals, and aggression]. Munich, Germany: Piper.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1984). Die Biologie des menschlichen Verhaltens: Grundriß der Humanethologie [The biology of human behavior: Outline of human ethology]. Munich, Germany: Piper.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1988). Der Mensch – das riskierte Wesen: Zur Naturgeschichte menschlicher Unvernunft [On the natural history of human folly]. Munich, Germany: Piper.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1997). Die Biologie menschlichen Verhaltens – Grundriss der Humanethologie [Human ethology – Foundations of human behavior]. Munich, Germany: Piper.Google Scholar
Eimer, M. (1987). Konzepte von Kausalität [Concepts of causality]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I., Liebermann, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1996). Detrimental effects of reward: Reality or myth?American Psychologist, 51, 1153–1166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1998). Reward, intrinsic interest, and creativity: New findings. American Psychologist, 53, 676–679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in the facial expressions of emotion. In Cole, J. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1971 (pp. 207–283). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 124–129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elias, N. (1969). Über den Prozess der Zivilisation: Soziogenetische und psychogenetische Untersuchungen [On the process of civilization: Sociogenetic and psychogenetic investigations]. Berne, Switzerland: Francke.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. (1997). Integrating the “classic” and “contemporary” approaches to achievement motivation: A hierarchical model of achievement motivation. In Maehr, M. & Pintrich, P. (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10, pp. 243–279). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, 169–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliot, A. (2005). A conceptual history of the achievement goal construct. In Elliot, A. J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 52–72). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 218–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. (1996). Approach and avoidance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 461–475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2×2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501–519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliot, E. S., & Dweck, C. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 5–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, M. H. (1928). The effect of change of reward on the maze performance of rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 19–30.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An approach to personality and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1058–1068.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (1992). Abstract versus concrete goals: Personal striving level, physical illness and psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 292–300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emmons, R. A. (2003). Personal goals, life meaning and virtue: Wellsprings of a positive life. In Keyes, C. L. & Haidt, J. (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 105–128). Washington, DC: American Psychological Associaton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmons, R. A., & McAdams, D. (1991). Personal strivings and motive dispositions: Exploring the links. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 648–654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engeser, S. (2004). Lernmotivation und volitionale Handlungssteuerung: Eine Längsschnittstudie beim Statistik Lernen im Psychologiestudium [Learning motivation and volitional action regulation: A longitudinal study on learning elementary statistics]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Potsdam, Germany.Google Scholar
Engeser, S., Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Bischoff, J. (2005). Motivation, Flow-Erleben und Lernleistung in universitären Lernsettings [Motivation, flow-experience, and performance in learning settings at universities]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 19, 159–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Entin, E. E. (1974). Effects of achievement-oriented and affiliative motives on private and public performance. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 219–236). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Entwisle, D. R. (1972). To dispel fantasies about fantasy-based measures of achievement motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 377–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Entwistle, D., & Hayduk, L. (1978). Too great expectations: Young children's academic outlook. Baltimore: The John Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Epstein, L. H., Truesdale, R., Wojcik, A., Paluch, R. A., & Raynor, H. A. (2003). Effects of deprivation on hedonics and reinforcing value of food. Physiology & Behavior, 78, 221–227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, S. (1962). The measurement of drive and conflict in humans: Theory and experiment. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1962 (pp. 127–206). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709–724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 390–405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.). (1996). The road to excellence. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society (Rev. ed.). New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Eron, L. D. (1994). Theories of aggression. From drives to cognitions. In Huesmann, L. R. (Ed.), Aggressive behaviour: Current perspectives (pp. 3–11). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Escalona, S. K. (1940). The effect of success and failure upon the level of aspiration and behavior in manic-depressive psychoses. University of Iowa Studies: Child Welfare, 16, 199–302.Google Scholar
Estes, W. K. (1958). Stimulus-response theory of drive. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1958 (pp. 35–69). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Everitt, B. J. (1990). Sexual motivation: A neural and behavioural analysis of the mechanisms underlying appetitive and copulatory responses of male rats. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 14(2), 217–232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Exline, R. V. (1962). Need affiliation and initial communication behavior in problem solving groups characterized by low interpersonal visibility. Psychological Reports, 10, 79–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of personality. In Pervin, L. (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 244–276). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Falbo, T. (1975). Achievement attributions of kindergarteners. Developmental Psychology, 11, 529–530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fallo-Mitchell, L., & Ryff, C. D. (1982). Preferred timing of female life events. Research on Aging, 4, 249–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fajans, S. (1933). Die Bedeutung der Entfernung für die Stärke eines Aufforderungscharakters beim Säugling und Kleinkind [The meaning of distance for the degree of variance as experienced by the infant]. Psychologische Forschung, 17, 215–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farr, J. L., Hofmann, D. A., & Mathieu, J. E. (1993). Job perception, job satisfaction relations: An empirical comparison of three competing theories. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision, 56(3), 370–387.Google Scholar
Fazey, J. A., & Hardy, L. (1988). The inverted-U hypothesis: A catastrophe for sport psychology. In British Association for Sports Sciences Monograph no. 1. Leeds, England.
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feather, N. T. (1959a). Subjective probability and decision under uncertainty. Psychological Review, 66, 150–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1959b). Success probability and choice behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 257–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1961). The relationship of persistence at a task to expectation of success and achievement related motives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 552–561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1962). The study of persistence. Psychological Bulletin, 59, 94–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1963b). The relationship of expectation of success to reporter probability, task structure and achievement-related motivation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 231–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1967). Valence of outcome and expectation of success in relation to task difficulty and perceived locus of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 372–386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feather, N. T. (Ed.). (1982). Expectations and actions: Expectancy-value models in psychology.Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Feld, S. C. (1967). Longitudinal study of the origins of achievement strivings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 408–414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, N. S., Higgins, E. T., Karlovac, M., & Ruble, D. N. (1976). Use of consensus information in causal attribution as a function of temporal presentation and availability of direct information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 694–698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenz, W. D. (1975). Strategies for coping with stress. In Sarason, I. G. & Spielberger, C. D. (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 2, pp. 305–336). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Ferguson, E. D. (1962). Ego involvement: A critical examination of some methodological issues. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64, 407–417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feshbach, S., & Singer, R. D. (1971). Television and aggression: An experimental field study.San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1942). A theoretical interpretation of shifts in level of aspiration. Psychological Review, 49, 235–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1964). Conflict, decision, and dissonance.Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 203–210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956). When prophecy fails.Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K. (1982). Causal schemata: Review and criticism of research on a popular construct. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 1001–1013.Google Scholar
Fiedler, K., & Bless, H. (2002). Soziale Kognition [Social cognition]. In Stroebe, W., Jonas, K., & Hewstone, M. (Eds.), Sozialpsychologie (4th ed., pp. 125–164). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fincham, F., & Jaspars, J. (1979). Attribution of responsibility to the self and other in children and adults. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1589–1602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fineman, S. (1977). The achievement motive construct and its measurement: Where are we now?British Journal of Psychology, 68, 1–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finkel, E. J., & Campbell, W. K. (2001). Self-control and accommodation in close relationships: An interdependence analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 265–277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkelstein, N. W., & Ramey, C. T. (1977). Learning to control the environment in infancy. Child Development, 48, 806–819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisch, R. (1970). Konfliktmotivation und Examen [Conflict motivation and examinations]. Meisenheim, Germany: Hain.Google Scholar
Fishbach, A. U., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not into temptations: Momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 296–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fishman, D. B. (1966). Need and expectancy as determinants of affiliative behavior in small groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 155–164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, D. W., & Maddi, S. R. (1961). A conceptual framework. In Fiske, D. W. & Maddi, S. R. (Eds.), Functions of varied experience (pp. 11–56). Homewood, IL: Dorsey.Google Scholar
Fitch, G. (1970). Effects of self-esteem, perceived performance, and choice on causal attributions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 311–315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance.Oxford, UK: Brooks(Cole.Google Scholar
Flammer, A., Züblin, C., & Grob, A. (1988). Sekundäre Kontrolle bei Jugendlichen [Secondary control in adolescents]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 20, 239–262.Google Scholar
Fleming, A. S., Corter, C., Franks, P., Surbey, M., Schneider, B., & Steiner, M. (1993). Postpartum factors related to mother's attraction to newborn infant odors. Developmental Psychobiology, 26(2), 115–132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, E. M. (1984). The power motive and reactivity to power stresses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 853–859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, E. M. (1985). The power motive. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1408–1416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, E. M., & Carver, R. A. (2000). Achievement and power motives, performance feedback, and creativity. Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 380–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, E. M., & Farrow, D. L. (1979). The power motive as an influence on use of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2091–2097.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, E. M., & Smith, T. (1982). The power motive as an influence on group decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 178–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. A. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-age community sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 21, 219–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). Ways of coping questionnaire: Manual.Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Forrest, D. W. (1959). The role of muscular tension in the recall of interrupted tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 181–184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Förster, J., & Liberman, N. (2002, June). Introducing a motivational priming model. Paper presented at the 13th general meeting of the European Association of Experimental Social Psychology, San Sebastian, Spain.Google Scholar
Försterling, F. (1989). Models of covariation and attribution: How do they relate to the analogy of analysis of variance?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(4), 615–625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Försterling, F. (2001). Attribution: An introduction to theories, research and applications. Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Försterling, F., Bühner, F., & Gall, S. (1998). Attributions of depressed persons: How consistant are they with the covariation principle?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 1047–1061.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ford, M. E., & Nicholls, C. W. (1987). A taxonomy of human goals and some possible applications. In Ford, M. E. & Ford, D. H. (Eds.), Humans as self-constructing systems: Putting the framework to work (pp. 289–311). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Forrest, D. W. (1959). The role of muscular tension in the recall of interrupted tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 181–184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, H. (1971). Implications of sensory reinforcement. In Glaser, R. (Ed.), The nature of reinforcement (pp. 151–195). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1988). Patterns of brain electrical activity during facial signs of emotion in 10-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 24, 230–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, K., Goudas, M, Biddle, S., Duda, J. L., & Armstrong, N. (1994). Children's task and ego goal profiles in sport. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, 253–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, J. D. (1935). Individual differences in certain aspects of the level of aspiration. American Journal of Psychology, 47, 119–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freedman, J. L. (1965) Preference for dissonant information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 287–289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, G. L. (1930). Changes in tonus during completed and interrupted mental work. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 4, 309–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freitas, A. L., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2002). Regulatory fit and temptation during goal pursuit. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 291–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
French, E. G. (1955). Some characteristics of achievement motivation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 232–236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, E. G. (1956). Motivation as a variable in workpartner selection. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53, 96–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, E. G. (1958a). Development of a measure of complex motivation. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 242–248). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
French, E. G. (1958b). Effects of the interaction of motivation and feedback on task performance. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 400–408). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
French, E. G., & Chadwick, I. (1956). Some characteristics of affiliation motivation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 296–300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, E. G., & Lesser, G. S. (1964). Some characteristics of the achievement motive in women. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 119–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, J. A., Kamil, A. C., & Leger, D. (Eds.). (2001). Evolutionary psychology and motivation. In Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 47 (p. 221). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
French, J. R. P., & Raven B. H. (1959). The basis of social power. In Cartwright, D. (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 150–167). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1895). Entwurf einer Psychologie [Outline of a psychological science]. In: Freud, S. (Ed.), From the beginnings of psychology (pp. 370–466). London: Imago Publishing.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1901). Zur Psychopathologie des Alltagslebens [The psychopathology of everyday life]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. IV (1952). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1915). Triebe und Triebschicksale [Drives and their fate]. Gesammelte Werke, Bd. X. Frankfurt: Fischer, 1952.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1926). Hemmung, Symptom, Angst [Inhibition, symptom, anxiety]. Gesammelte Werke, Bd. XIV. Frankfurt: Fischer, 1952.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1952). Die Abwehrneuropsychosen [The neuropsychoses of defense]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. I (1894). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1952). Die Traumdeutung [The interpretation of dreams]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. II–III (1900). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1952). Hemmung, Symptom, Angst [Inhibition, symptoms and anxiety]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. ⅪV (1926). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1952). Triebe und Triebschicksale [Instincts and their vicissitudes]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. X (1915). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1961). Drei Abhandlungen zur Sexualtheorie [Three essays on the theory of sexuality]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. V (3rd ed.) (1905). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1963). Das Unbehagen in der Kultur [Civilization and its discontents]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. ⅪV (3rd ed.) (1930). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). The orchestration of selection, optimization and compensation: An action-theoretical conceptualization of a theory of developmental regulation. In Perrig, W. J. & Grob, A. (Eds.), Control of human behavior, mental processes, and consciousness: Essays in honor of the 60th birthday of August Flammer (pp. 35–58). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Frey, D. (1981). Informationssuche und Informationsbewertung bei Entscheidungen [The search for and evaluation of information in decision making]. Stuttgart, Germany: Huber.Google Scholar
Frey, D., & Irle, M. (1972). Some conditions to produce a dissonance and an incentive effect in a “forced-compliance” situation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 45–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frey, K. S., & Ruble, D. N. (1985). What children say when the teacher is not around: Conflicting goals in social comparison and performance assessment in the classroom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 550–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frey, R. S. (1984). Does n-achievement cause economic development? A cross-lagged panel analysis of the McClelland thesis. Journal of Social Psychology, 122, 67–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, J. M., & Halaas, J. L. (1998). Leptin and the regulation of body weight in mammals. Nature, 395(6704), 763–770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fries, S. (2002). Wollen und Können [Volition and ability]. Münster, Germany: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Fries, S., Lund, B., & Rheinberg, F. (1999). Lässt sich das Training induktiven Denkens durch gleichzeitige Motivförderung optimieren? [Does simultaneous motive modification optimise the teaching of inductive reasoning?]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 13, 37–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43, 249–358.CrossRef
Fuchs, R. (1954). Gewißheit, Motivation und bedingter Reflex [Certainty, motivation, and conditioned reflex]. Meisenheim, Germany: Hain.Google Scholar
Fuhrmann, A., & Kuhl, J. (1998). Maintaining a healthy diet: Effects of personality and self-reward versus self-punishment on commitment to and enactment of self-chosen and assigned goals. Psychology and Health, 13, 651–686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuster, J. M. (2001). The prefrontal cortex – an update: Time is of the essence. Neuron, 30(2), 319–333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fyans, L. J., Salili, M., Maehr, M. L., & Desai, K. A. (1983). A cross-cultural exploration into the meaning of achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1000–1013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gagné, F. M., & Lydon, J. E. (2001a). Mind-set and close relationships: When bias leads to (in)accurate predictions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 85–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gagné, F. M., & Lydon, J. E. (2001b). Mindset and relationship illusions: The moderating effects of domain specificity and relationship commitment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1144–1155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gagné, F. M., Lydon, J. E., & Bartz, J. A. (2003). Effects of mindset on the predictive validity of relationship constructs. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 35, 292–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galbraith, J., & Cummings, L. (1967). An empiric investigation of the motivational determinants of past performance: Interactive effects between instrumentality-valence, motivation, and ability. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 2, 237–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Magee, J. C. (2003). From power to action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 453–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Garland, H. (1984). Relation of effort-performance expectancy to performance in goal-setting experiments. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 79–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaugele, H., & Ullmer, C. (1990). Zur Anreizstruktur des Bodybuildings [The incentive structure of body building]. Unpublished manuscript, University of Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Gavin, J. F. (1970). Ability, effort, and role perception as antecedents of job performance. Experimental Publication System (Manuscript no. 190A), 5.Google Scholar
Gawrilow, C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2004, April). Inhibition fortlaufender Reaktionen durch Vorsätze bei ADHS Kindern [Inhibition of continuous reactions in ADHD children]. Paper presented at the symposium “Recent developments in research on implementation intentions” at the 46th meeting of the Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen (TeaP), Giessen, Germany.Google Scholar
Gebhard, M. (1948). Effects of success and failure upon the attractiveness of activities as a function of experience, expectation, and need. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 371–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gehlen, A. (1958). Der Mensch: Seine Natur und seine Stellung in der Welt [The human being: His nature and status in the world]. Bonn, Germany: Athenäum.Google Scholar
Georgopolous, B. S., Mahony, G. M., & Jones, N. W. (1957). A path-goal approach to productivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 41, 345–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geppert, U., & Gartmann, D. (1983, July). The emergence of self evaluative emotions as consequences of achievement actions. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Munich, Germany.Google Scholar
Geppert, U., & Heckhausen, H. (1990). Ontogenese der Emotion [Ontogenesis of emotion]. In Scherer, K. R. (Ed.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Psychologie der Emotion (Vol. IV, No. 3, pp. 115–213). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Geppert, U., & Küster, U. (1983). The emergence of “wanting to do it oneself”: A precursor of achievement motivation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 6, 355–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gianotti, M., Roca, P., & Palou, A. (1988). Body weight and tissue composition in rats made obese by a cafeteria diet: Effect of 24 hours starvation. Hormone and Metabolic Research, 20(4), 208–212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Adaptive thinking: Rationality in the real world. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & the ABC Research Group. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gilligan, S. G. (1997). The courage to love: Principles and practices of self-relations psychotherapy.New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Gjesme, T. (1971). Motive to achieve success and motive to avoid failure in relation to school performance for pupils of different ability levels. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 15, 81–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gjesme, T., & Nygard, R. (1970). Achievement-related motives: Theoretical considerations and construction of a measuring instrument. Unpublished manuscript, University of Oslo, Norway.Google Scholar
Glixman, A. F. (1948). An analysis of the use of the interruption-technique in experimental studies of “repression.”Psychological Bulletin, 45, 491–506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, L. R. (1982). From ace to zombie: Some explorations in the language of personality. In Spielberger, C. D. & Butcher, J. N. (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 1, pp. 203–234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goleman, D. (1994). Emotional intelligence. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Suchen, Finden und Festigen der eigenen Identität: Unstillbare Zielintentionen [Searching, finding, and consolidating of one's own identity: Unsaturatable goal intentions]. In Heckhausen, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Weinert, F. E. (Eds.), Jenseits des Rubikon: Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften (pp. 176–189). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 53–92). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1991). Abwägen und Planen [Consideration and planning]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: the role of intentions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 141–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1998). Implicit and explicit processes in goal pursuit. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society of Experimental Social Psychology, Lexington, KY.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 186–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (2003). Why we thought that action mind-sets affect illusions of control. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 261–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.). (1996). The psychology of action:Linking cognition and motivation to behavior. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bayer, U. (1999). Deliberative versus implemental mindsets in the control of action. In Chaiken, S. & Trope, Y. (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 403–422). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bayer, U. (2000). Becoming a better person without changing yourself. Paper presented at the Self and Identity Pre-conference of the annual meeting of the Society of Experimental Social Psychology, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Bayer, U., & McCulloch, C. (2003). The control of the unwanted. In Hassin, R., Uleman, J., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), The new unconscious (pp. 485–515). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Bayer, U., Steller, B., & Bargh, J. A. (2002). Delegating control to the environment: Perception, attention and memory for preselected behavioral cues. Unpublished manuscript, University of Konstanz, Germany.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 186–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Fujita, K., & Oettingen, G. (2004). Planning and the implementation of goals. In Baumeister, R. & Vohs, K. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation(pp. 211–228). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Ratajczak, H. (1990). From weighing to willing: Approaching a change decision through pre- or postdecisional mentation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 45, 41–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Steller, B. (1990). Deliberative and implemental mind-sets: Cognitive tuning toward congruous thoughts and information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1119–1127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kirchhof, O. (1998). The willful pursuit of identity. In Heckhausen, J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulation across the life span (pp. 389–423). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kinney, R. F. (1989). Effects of deliberative and implemental mind-sets on illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 531–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, G. B. (1996). Goal effects on action and cognition. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 361–399). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Schaal, B. (1998). Metacognition in action: The importance of implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 124–136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollwitzer, P. M., Trötschel, R., Bayer, U., & Sumner, M. (2004). Potential costs of self-regulation by implementation intentions: Rebound and ego-depletion effects? Unpublished manuscript, University of Konstanz, Germany.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Wicklund, R. A. (1985a). Self-symbolizing and the neglect of others' perspectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 702–715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Wicklund, A. (1985b). The pursuit of self-defining goals. Action control: from cognition to behavior (pp. 61–85). Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
Goodenough, F. L. (1931). Anger in young children. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, P. S., Rose, J. H., & Furcon, J. E. (1970). Comparison of motivational antecedents of the work performance of scientists and engineers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 54, 491–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodstadt, B. E., & Hjelle, L. A. (1973). Power to the powerless: Locus of control and the use of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 190–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goschke, T. (1997a). Zur Funktionsanalyse des Willens: Integration kognitions-, motivations- und neuropsychologischer Perspektiven [On the functional architecture of volitional action: Integration of cognitive, motivational, and neuropsychological perspectives]. Psychologische Beiträge, 39, 375–412.Google Scholar
Goschke, T. (1997b). Implicit learning of perceptual and motor sequences: Evidence for independent learning systems. In Stadler, M. & French, P. (Eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 401–444). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (1993). The representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1211–1226.Google Scholar
Gottfredson, L. S. (1981). Circumscription and compromise: A developmental theory of occupational aspirations. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 28(6), 545–579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behavioral development theory. Developmental Psychology, 27, 4–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottschaldt, K. (1926). Über den Einfluss der Erfahrung auf die Wahrnehmung von Figuren [On the influence of past experience on perception]. Psychologische Forschung, 8, 261–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gough, H. G. (1990). The California Psychological Inventory. In Watkins, C. E. & Campbell, V. L. (Eds.), Testing in counselling practice (pp. 37–62). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Graen, G. (1969). Instrumentality theory of work motivation: Some experimental results and suggested modifications. Journal of Applied Psychology Monographs, 53, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J. M., & Desjardins, C. (1980). Classical conditioning: Induction of luteinizing hormone and testosterone secretion in anticipation of sexual activity. Science, 210, 1039–1041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, S., & Hudley, C. (1994). Attributions of aggressive and nonaggressive African-American male early adolescents: A study of construct accessibility. Developmental Psychology, 30, 365–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, S., Hudley, C., & Williams, E. (1992). Attributional and emotional determinants of aggression among African-American and Latino young adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 28, 731–740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, S., Weiner, B., & Benesh-Weiner, M. (1995). An attributional analysis of the development of excuse giving in aggressive and nonaggressive African American boys. Developmental Psychology, 31(2), 274–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1971). The psychology of fear and stress. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck's theory of personality. In Eysenck, H. J. (Ed.), A model for personality (pp. 246–276). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1982). The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Graziano, W. G., & Eisenberg, N. H. (1997). Agreeableness: A dimension of personality. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 793–825). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Green, D. (1963). Volunteering and the recall of interrupted tasks. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 397–401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. M., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., & Maynard, A. (2002). Cultural pathways through universal development. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 461–490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenough, A., Cole, G., Lewis, J., Lockton, A., & Blundell, J. (1998). Untangling the effects of hunger, anxiety, and nausea on energy intake during intravenous cholecystokinin octapeptide (cck-8) infusion. Physiology & Behavior, 65(2), 303–310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G. (1982). Ego task analysis: An integration of research on ego-involvement and self-awareness. In Hastorf, A. & Isen, A. (Eds.), Cognitive social psychology (pp. 109–147). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109, 3–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greve, W., & Wentura, D. (2003). Immunizing the self: Self-concept stabilization through reality-adaptive self-definitions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 39–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grinker, J. (1969). Cognitive control of classical eyelid conditioning. In Zimbardo, P. G. (Ed.), The cognitive control of motivation (pp. 126–135). Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
Grob, A., Krings, F., & Bangerter, A. (2001). Life markers in biographical narratives of people from three cohorts: A life span perspective in its historical context. Human Development, 44(4), 171–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Groeben, N., Wahl, D., Schlee, J., & Scheele, B. (1988). Das Forschungsprogramm Subjektive Theorien. Eine Einführung in die Psychologie des reflexiven Subjekts [Research on subjective theories. An introduction to the psychology of the reflexive subject]. Tübingen, Germany: Francke.Google Scholar
Groos, K. (1899). Die Spiele des Menschen [The play of man]. Jena, Germany: Fischer.
Gubler, H., Paffrath, M., & Bischof, N. (1994). Untersuchungen zur Systemanalyse der sozialen Motivation III: Eine Aestimationsstudie zur Sicherheits- und Erregungsregulation während der Adoleszenz [An estimation study of security and arousal regulation during adolescence]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 202, 95–132.Google Scholar
Guevara, M. L. (1994). Alienation und Selbstkontrolle: Das Ignorieren eigener Gefühle [Alienation and self-control: Ignoring one's own feelings]. Berne, Switzerland: Lang.Google Scholar
Guinote, A., Judd, C. M., & Brauer, M. (2002). Effects of power on perceived and objective group variability: Evidence that more powerful groups are more variable. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 708–721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gupta, B. S., & Nagpal, M. (1978). Impulsivity(sociability and reinforcement in verbal operant conditioning. British Journal of Psychology, 69, 203–206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurack, E. (1978). Die Entwicklung des Fähigkeitskonzepts im Vorschulalter [The development of the ability concept at preschool age]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Haase, C. M., Heckhausen, J., & Köller, O. (2007). Goal engagement during the school-work transition: Beneficial for all, particularly for girls. Journal of Research on Adolescence.Google Scholar
Haber, R. N. (1958). Discrepancy from adaptation level as a source of affect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 370–375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haber, R. N., & Alpert, R. (1958). The role of situation and picture cues in projective measurement of the achievement motive. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 644–663). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Hackman, J. R., & Porter, L. W. (1968). Expectancy theory predictions of work effectiveness. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 417–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haffner, S. (2003). Historische Variationen [Historical variations]. Munich, Germany: dtv.Google Scholar
Haider, M. (1969). Elektrophysiologische Indikatoren der Aktiviertheit [Electrophysiological indicators of activation]. In Schönpflug, W. (Ed.), Methoden der Aktivierungsforschung (pp. 125–156). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Halisch, C., & Halisch, F. (1980). Kognitive Voraussetzungen frühkindlicher Selbstbewertungsreaktionen nach Erfolg und Misserfolg [Cognitive prerequisites of self-evaluative reactions after success and failure in early childhood]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 12, 193–212.Google Scholar
Halisch, F. (1986). Operante und respondente Verfahren zur Messung des Leistungsmotivs [Operant and respondent measures for the assessment of need for achievement]. Munich, Germany: Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research.Google Scholar
Halisch, F., & Geppert, U. (2001a). Motives, personal goals, and life satisfaction in old age: First results from the Munich Twin Study (GOLD). In Trends and prospects in motivation research (pp. 389–409). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Halisch, F., & Geppert, U. (2001b). Genetic vs. environmental determinants of traits, motives, self-referential cognitions, and volitional control in old age: First results from the Munich Twin Study (GOLD). In Trends and prospects in motivation research (pp. 359–387). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Halisch, F., & Heckhausen, H. (1977). Search for feedback information and effort regulation during task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 724–733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halisch, F., & Heckhausen, H. (1988). Motive-dependent vs. ability-dependent valence functions for success and failure. In Halisch, F. & Bercken, J. (Eds.), Intentional perspectives on achievement and task motivation. Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Halisch, F., & Kuhl, J. (Eds.). (1986). Motivation, intention, and volition. Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
Hamburg, D. A. (1963). Emotions in the perspective of human evolution. In Knapp, P. H. (Ed.), Expression of emotions in man (pp. 300–317). New York: International University Press.Google Scholar
Hamilton, J. O. (1974). Motivation and risk-taking behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 856–864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, V. L. (1980). Intuitive psychologist or intuitive lawyer?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 767–772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 17–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, R. D., & Stonner, D. M. (1978). Attributes and attributions: Inferring stimulus properties, actors' dispositions, and causes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 657–667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., & Elliot, A. (1998). Rethinking achievement goals: When are they adaptive for college students and why?Educational Psychologist, 33(1), 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., & Elliot, A. (2002). Predicting succes in college: A longituinal study of achievement goals and ability measure as predictors of interest and performance from freshman year through graduation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 562–575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harackiewicz, J. M., & Elliot, A. (1993). Achievement goals and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 904–915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harcourt, A. H. (1987). Dominance and fertility among female primates. Journal of Zoology, 213, 471–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harcourt, A. H. (1989). Social influences on competitive ability: Alliances and their consequences. In Standen, V. & Foley, R. A. (Eds.), Comparative socioecology – The behavioural ecology of humans and other mammals (pp. 223–242). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hardy, K. R. (1957). Determinants of conformity and attitude change. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 289–294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardyck, J. A., & Braden, M. (1962). Prophecy fails again: A report of a failure to replicate. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 136–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1950). Learning and satiation of response in intrinsically motivated complex puzzle performance by monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 289–294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, H. F. (1953). Motivation as a factor in the acquisition of new responses. In Current theory and research in motivation: A symposium. (pp. 24–49). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1971). Learning to love.San Francisco: Abbion.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F., & Harlow, M. H. (1966). Learning how to care. American Scientist, 54, 244–272.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F., & Zimmermann, R. (1959). Affectional responses in the infant monkey. Science, 130, 421–432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmon-Jones, E. (2000). Cognitive dissonance and experienced negative affect: Evidence that dissonance increases experienced negative affect even in the absence of aversive consequences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1490–1501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2002). Testing the action-based model of cognitive dissonance: The effect of action-orientation on post-decisional attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 711–723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, B. (1979). Whatever happened to little Albert?American Psychologist, 34, 151–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child's environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102, 458–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harter, S. (1974). Pleasure derived from cognitive challenge and mastery. Child Development, 45, 661–669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered. Human Development, 21, 34–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartshorne, H., & May, M. A. (1928). Studies in the nature of character (Vol. 1). Studies in deceit.New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hartshorne, H., & May, M. A. (1929). Studies in the nature of character (Vol. 2). Studies in service and self-control.New York: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hautzinger, M. (1994). Action control in the context of psychopathological disorders. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 209–215). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Havighurst, R. J. (1953). Human development and education. London: Longmans.Google Scholar
Hayashi, T., & Yamauchi, K. (1964). The relation of children's need for achievement to their parents' home discipline in regard to independence and mastery. Bulletin of Kyoto Gakugei University, A25, 31–40.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1946). Onthe nature of fear. Psychological Review, 53, 259–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1953). Heredity and environment in mammalian behavior. British Journal of Animal Behavior, 1, 43–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1955). Drives and the C. N. S. (conceptual nervous system). Psychological Review, 62, 243–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1955). Motivationsanalyse der Anspruchsniveau-Setzung [Motivational analysis of the setting of levels of aspiration]. Psychologische Forschung, 25, 118–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1960). Die Problematik des Projektionsbegriffs und die Grundlagen und Grundannahmen des Thematischen Auffassungstests [The problems surrounding the concept of projection and the basic assumptions of the Thematic Apperception Test]. Psychologische Beiträge, 5, 53–80.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1963a). Hoffnung und Furcht in der Leistungsmotivation [Hope and fear in achievement motivation]. Meisenheim(Glan, Germany: Hain.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1963b). Eine Rahmentheorie der Motivation in zehn Thesen [A framework theory of motivation in ten theses]. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 10, 604–626.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1964a). Über die Zweckmäßigkeit einiger Situationsbedingungen bei der inhaltsanalytischen Erfassung der Motivation [On the suitability of some situational conditions in the content-based assessment of motivation]. Psychologische Forschung, 27, 244–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1964b). Entwurf einer Psychologie des Spielens. Psychologische Forschung, 27, 225–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1965). Leistungsmotivation [Achievement motivation]. In Thomae, H. (Ed.), Handbuch der Psychologie (Vol. II, pp. 602–702). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1967). The anatomy of achievement motivation.New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1968). Achievement motive research: Current problems and some contributions toward a general theory of motivation. In Arnold, W. J. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1968 (pp. 103–174). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1969). Allgemeine Psychologie in Experimenten [General psychology in experiments]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1972). Die Interaktion der Sozialisationsvariablen in der Genese des Leistungsmotivs [The interaction of socialization variables in the genesis of the achievement motive]. In Graumann, C. F. (Ed.), Handbuch der Psychologie (Vol. 7/2, pp. 955–1019). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1973). Intervening cognitions in motivation. In Berlyne, D. & Madsen, K. B. (eds.), Pleasure, reward, and preference (pp. 217–242). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1974a). Leistung und Chancengleichheit [Achievement and equality of opportunity]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1974b). Motivationsanalysen [Analyses of motivation]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1975a). Effort expenditure, aspiration level and self-evaluation before and after unexpected performance shifts. Unpublished manuscript, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1975b). Fear of failure as a self-reinforcing motive system. In Sarason, I. G. & Spielberger, C. (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. II, pp. 117–128). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1977a). Achievement motivation and its constructs: A cognitive model. Motivation and Emotion, 1(4), 283–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1977b). Motivation: Kognitionspsychologische Aufspaltung eines summarischen Konstrukts [Motivation: Splitting a summary construct along the lines of cognitive psychology]. Psychologische Rundschau, 28, 175–189.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1978). Selbstbewertung nach erwartungswidrigem Leistungsverlauf: Einfluß von Motiv, Kausalattribution und Zielsetzung [Self-evaluation after unexpected performances: Influence of motives, causal attributions, and goal selection]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 10, 191–216.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1980). Motivation und Handeln [Motivation and action]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1981). Neuere Entwicklungen in der Motivationsforschung [Recent developments in motivation research]. In Michaelis, W. (Ed.), Bericht über den 32. Kongreß der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie, Zürich 1980 (pp. 325–335). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1982). The development of achievement motivation. In Hartup, W. W. (Ed.), Review of child development research (pp. 600–668). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1983a). Motivationsmodelle: Fortschreitende Entfaltung und unbehobene Mängel [Models of motivation: Progressive development and remaining shortcomings]. In Hacker, W., Volpert, W., & Cranach, M. V. (Eds.), Kognitive und motivationale Aspekte der Handlung (pp. 9–17). Berlin, Germany: VEB Deutscher Verlag der Wissenschaften.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1983b). Entwicklungsschritte in der Kausalattribution von Handlungsergebnissen [Developmental stages in the causal attribution of action outcomes]. Kindliche Erklärungsmuster: Entwicklungspsychologische Beiträge zur Attributionsforschung, 1, 49–85.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1984a). Attibutionsmuster für Leistungsergebnisse: Individuelle Unterschiede, mögliche Arten und deren Genese [Attribution patterns for performance outcomes: Individual differences, possible types, and their development]. In Weinert, F. E. & Kluwe, R. H. (Eds.), Metakognition, Motivation und Lernen (pp. 133–164). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1984b). Emergent achievement behavior: Some early developments. In Nicholls, J. (Ed.), Advances in achievement motivation (pp. 1–32). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1987a). Wünschen – Wählen – Wollen [Wishing – weighing – willing]. In Heckhausen, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Weinert, F. E. (Eds.), Jenseits des Rubikon: Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften (pp. 3–9). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1987b). Perspektiven einer Psychologie des Wollens [Perspectives on a psychology of the will]. In Heckhausen, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Weinert, F. E. (Eds.), Jenseits des Rubikon: Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften (pp. 121–142). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1987c). Vorsatz, Wille und Bedürfnis: Lewins frühes Vermächtnis und ein zugeschütteter Rubikon [Intention, will, and need: Lewin's early legacy and a missed Rubicon]. In Heckhausen, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Weinert, F. E. (Eds.), Jenseits des Rubikon: Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften (pp. 86–96). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1989). Motivation und Handeln [Motivation and action] (2nd ed.). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and action. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents and cognitive functioning in motivational versus volitional states of mind. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 101–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Weinert, F. E. (Eds.). (1987). Jenseits des Rubikon: Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften [Beyond the Rubicon: The will in the behavioral and social sciences]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Halisch, F. (1986, July). “Operant” versus “respondent” motive measures: A problem of validity or of construct? Paper presented at the 21st International Congress of Applied Psychology, Jerusalem, Israel.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Kuhl, J. (1985). From wishes to action: The dead ends and short cuts on the long way to action. In Frese, M. & Sabini, L. (Eds.), Goal-directed behavior: Psychological theory and research on action (pp. 134–160, 367–395). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Meyer, W. U. (1972). Selbständigkeitserziehung und Leistungsmotiv [Raising children to be independent and the achievement motive]. Forschungsgemeinschaft, 4, 10–14.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Rheinberg, F. (1980). Lernmotivation im Unterricht, erneut betrachtet [A new look at motivation to learn in the classroom]. Unterrichtswissenschaft, 8, 7–47.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Roelofsen, I. (1962). Anfänge und Entwicklung der Leistungsmotivation: (I) Im Wetteifer des Kleinkindes [Origins and development of achievement motivation: (I) In the competition of infancy]. Psychologische Forschung, 26, 313–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H., Schmalt, H.-D., & Schneider, K. (1985). Achievement motivation in perspective. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Strang, H. (1988). Efficiency under maximal performance demands: Exertion control, an individual-difference variable?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 489–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Wagner, I. (1965). Anfänge der Entwicklung der Leistungsmotivation: (II) In der Zielsetzung des Kleinkindes. Zur Genese des Anspruchsniveaus [Origins and development of achievement motivation: (II) In the goal setting of infancy. Development of the level of aspiration]. Psychologische Forschung, 28, 179–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1987a). Balancing for weaknesses and challenging developmental potential: A longitudinal study of mother-infant dyads in apprenticeship interactions. Developmental Psychology, 23, 762–770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1987b). How do mothers know? Infants' chronological age or infants performance as determinants of adaptation in maternal instructions?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 43, 212–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1988). Becoming aware of ones competence in the second year: Developmental progression within the mother-child dyad. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11, 305–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1989). Normatives Entwicklungswissen als Bezugsrahmen zur (Re)Konstruktion der eigenen Biographie [Normative developmental knowledge as a framework for (re)constructing one's own biography]. In Alheit, P. & Hörning, E. (Eds.), Biographisches Wissen: Beiträge zu einer Theorie lebensgeschichtlicher Erfahrung (pp. 202–282). Frankfurt, Germany: Campus.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1990). Erwerb und Funktion normativer Vorstellungen über den Lebenslauf: Ein entwicklungspsychologischer Beitrag zur sozio-psychischen Konstruktion von Biographien [Acquisition and function of normative conceptions about the life course: A developmental psychology approach to the socio-psychological construction of biographies]. Kölner Zeitschrift für Soziologie und Sozialpsychologie, 31, 351–373.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1991). Adults' expectancies about development and its controllability: Enhancing self-efficacy by social comparisons. In Schwarzer, R. (Ed.), Self-efficacy: Thought control of action (pp. 107–126). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1993). The development of mastery and its perception within caretaker-child dyads. In Messer, D. (Ed.), Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and social processes (pp. 55–79). New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1997). Developmental regulation across adulthood: Primary and secondary control of age-related challenges. Developmental Psychology, 33, 176–187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckhausen, J. (1999). Developmental regulation in adulthood: Age-normative and sociostructural constraints as adaptive challenges. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (2000a). Evolutionary perspectives on human motivation. American Behavioral Scientist, 43, 1015–1029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (2000b). Developmental regulation across the life span: An action-phase model of engagement and disengagement with developmental goals. In Heckhausen, J. (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 213–231). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (2002a). Transition from school to work: Societal opportunities and the potential for individual agency. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60, 173–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (2002b). Developmental regulation of life-course transitions: A control theory approach. In Pulkkinen, L. & Caspi, A. (Eds.), Paths to successful development: Personality in the life course (pp. 257–280). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (2005). Competence and motivation in adulthood and old age: Making the most of changing capacities and resources. In Elliot, A. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 240–256). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1991). Perceived controllability of expected psychological change across adulthood and old age. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 46, 165–173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckhausen, J., & Brim, O. G. (1997). Perceived problems for self and others: Self-protection by social downgrading throughout adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 12, 610–619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckhausen, J., Chang, E. S., Chen, C., & Greenberger, E. (2007). Optimistic goal selection and control striving during the transition after high school. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Heckhausen, J., Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Gains and losses in development throughout adulthood as perceived by different adult age groups. Developmental Psychology, 25, 109–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Dweck, C. S. (1998).(Eds.). Life span perspectives on motivation and control. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Farruggia, S. P. (2003). Developmental regulation across the life span: A control-theory approach and implications for secondary education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, Monograph series II: Psychological aspects of education – Current trends. Leicester, UK: British Psychological Society.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Hosenfeld, B. (1988, October). Lebensspannenentwicklung von normativen Vorstellungen über Lebensspannenentwicklung [Life-span development of normative conceptions about life-span development]. Paper presented at the 36th Congress of the Deutsche Gesellschaft für Psychologie, Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Krüger, J. (1993). Developmental expectations for the self and most other people: Age grading in three functions of social comparison. Developmental Psychology, 29, 539–548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1993). Optimisation by selection and compensation: Balancing primary and secondary control in life span development. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 16(2), 287–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 102(2), 284–304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1999a). The primacy of primary control is a human universal: A reply to Gould's critique of the life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 106(3), 605–609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1999b). Biological and societal canalizations and individuals' developmental goals. In Brandstädter, J. & Lerner, R. (Eds.), Action and self development: Theory and research through the life span (pp. 67–103). London: Sage.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J., Schulz, R. & Wrosch, C. (1998). Developmental regulation in adulthood: Optimization in primary and secondary control – A multiscale questionnaire (OPS-Scales) [Technical report]. In Heckhausen, J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulation across the life span (pp. 50–77). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Tomasik, M. J. (2002). Get an apprenticeship before school is out: How German adolescents adjust vocational aspirations when getting close to a developmental deadline. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60, 199–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Fleeson, W. W. (2001). Developmental regulation before and after a developmental deadline: The sample case of “biological clock” for child-bearing. Psychology and Aging, 16, 400–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heider, F. (1946). Attitudes and cognitive organization. Journal of Psychology, 21, 107–112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations.New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heider, F. (1960). The Gestalt theory of motivation. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1960 (pp. 145–172). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard?Psychological Review, 106, 766–794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heinz, W. R. (1999). From education to work: Cross-national perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heinz, W. R., Krüger, H., Rettke, U., Wachtveitl, E., & Witzel, A. (1985). Hauptsache eine Lehrstelle: Jugendliche vor den Hürden des Arbeitsmarkts [The main thing is having an apprenticeship: Young people and the hurdles of the labor market]. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Held, T. (1986). Institutionalization and deinstitutionalization of the life course. Human Development, 29, 157–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmke, A. (1987). Entwicklung lern- und leistungsbezogener Motive und Einstellungen: Ergebnisse aus dem SCHOLASTIK-Projekt [Development of learning- and achievement-related motives and attitudes: Findings from the SCHOLASTIK project]. In Weinert, F. E. & Helmke, A. (Eds.), Entwicklung im Grundschulalter (pp. 59–76). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Helmke, A., & Weinert, F. E. (1997). Bedingungsfaktoren schulischer Leistungen [Conditional factors of school achievement]. In Weinert, F. E. (Ed.), Psychologie des Unterrichts und der Schule (Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Themenbereich D, Serie I: Pädagogische Psychologie) (Vol. 3, pp. 71–176). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Helson, H. (1948). Adaptation level as a basis for a quantitative theory of frames of reference. Psychological Review, 55, 297–313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory.New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Helson, H. (1973). A common model for affectivity and perception: An adaption-level approach. In Berlyne, D. E. & Madsen, K. B. (Eds.), Pleasure, reward, preference (pp. 167–188). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Henemann, H. H., & Schwab, D. P. (1972). Evaluation of research on expectancy theory of employee performance. Psychological Bulletin, 78, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henle, M. (1944). The influence of valence on substitution. Journal of Psychology, 17, 11–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hentsch, A. (1992). Motivationale Aspekte des Malens: Eine Anreizanalyse [Motivational aspects of painting: An analysis of incentives]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Hepper, P. G. (1994). Long-term retention of kinship recognition established during infancy in the domestic dog. Behavioural Processes, 33, 3–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermann, M. G. (1980). Assessing the personalities of Soviet politburo members. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 332–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermans, H. J. M. (1970). A questionnaire measure of achievement motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 54, 353–363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, E. H. (1962). Ethology. In Newcomb, T. M. (Ed.), New directions in psychology (Vol. I). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Hess, W. R. (1954). Das Zwischenhirn: Syndrome, Lokalisationen, Emotionen [The interbrain: Syndromes, localizations, emotions]. Basel, Switzerland: Schwabe.Google Scholar
Hesse, F. W., Spies, K., & Lüer, G. (1983). Einfluß motivationaler Faktoren auf das Problemlöseverhalten im Umgang mit komplexen Problemen [Influence of motivational factors on problem-solving behavior in the context of complex problems]. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 30, 400–424.Google Scholar
Hetzer, H. (1931). Kind und Schaffen: Experimente über konstuktive Betätigung im Kleinkindalter [Children and creative work: Experiments on constructive activity in young childhood]. Jena, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Heyman, D., Dweck, C. S., & Cain, L. (1992). Young children's vulnerability to self-blame and helplessness. Child Development, 63, 391–403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyman, D., Gee, C. L., & Giles, J. W. (2003). Preschool children's reasoning about ability. Child Development, 74, 516–534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyn, S., Schnabel, U., & Röder, P. M. (1997). Von der Options- zur Realitätslogik. Stabilität und Wandel berufsbezogener Wertvorstellungen in der Statuspassage Schule-Beruf [Stability and change of career-related values at the school-to-work transition]. In Meier, A., Rabe-Kleberg, U. & Rodax, K. (Eds.), Jahrbuch Bildung und Arbeit ‘97: Transformation und Tradition in Ost und West (S. 281–305). Opladen: Leske and Budrich.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyns, R. W., Veroff, J., & Atkinson, J. W. (1958). A scoring manual for the affiliation motive. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 205–218). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Hidi, S. (2000). An interest researcher's perspective: The effects of extrinsic and intrinsic factors on motivation. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 311–339). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hiemisch, A., Ehlers, A., & Westermann, R. (2002). Mindsets in social anxiety: A new look at selective information processing. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33, 103–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, E. T., & Silberman, I. (1998). Development of regulatory focus: Promotion and prevention as ways of living. In Heckhausen, J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Motivation and self regulation across the life span (pp. 78–113). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, R. L., Snyder, C. R., & Berglas, S. (Eds.). (1990). Self-handicapping: The paradox that isn't. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hillgruber, A. (1912). Fortlaufende Arbeit und Willensbetätigung [Continuous work and use of volition]. Untersuchungen zur Psychologie und Philosophie, 1, 6.Google Scholar
Himmi, T., Boyer, A., & Orsini, J. C. (1988). Changes in lateral hypothalamic neuronal activity accompanying hyper- and hypoglycemias. Physiology & Behavior, 44, 347–354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinde, R. A. (1974). The study of aggression: Determinants, consequences, goals, and functions. In Wit, J. & Hartup, W. W. (Eds.), Determinants and origins of aggressive behavior (pp. 3–27). The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton.Google Scholar
Hiroto, D. W., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Generality of learned helplessness in man. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 311–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodoka, A., & Fincham, F. D. (1995). Origins of children's helpless and mastery achievement patterns in the family. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 375–385.Google Scholar
Hofer, J., & Chasiotis, A. (2004). Methodological considerations of applying a TAT-type picture-story test in cross-cultural research: A comparison of German and Zambian adolescents. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 224–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis: Die Funktion von Antizipationen in der menschlichen Verhaltenssteuerung und Wahrnehmung [Prediction and insight: The function of anticipation in human behavioral control and perception]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Hoffmann, N. (1998). Zwänge und Depressionen: Pierre Janet und die Verhaltenstherapie [Compulsions and depressions: Pierre Janet and behavioral therapy]. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogan, J., & Hogan, J. (1995). Hogan Personality Inventory Manual. Tulsa, OK: Hogan Assessment Systems.Google Scholar
Hogan, J., & Ones, D. S. (1997). Conscientiousness and integrity at work. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 849–870). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hogan, R. (1996). A socioanalytic perspective on the five-factor model. In Wiggins, J. S. (Ed.), The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 163–179). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hogan, R. (1981). Transitions and the life course. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hokoda, A., & Fincham, F. D. (1995). Origins of children's helpless and mastery achievement patterns in the family. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 375–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holder, W. B., Marx, M. N., Holder, E. E., & Collier, G. (1957). Response strength as a function of delay in a runway. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 316–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holland, J. L. (1997). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work environments. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Holowka, S., & Petitto, L. A. (2002). Left hemisphere cerebral specialization for babies with babbling. Science, 297, 1515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoppe, F. (1930). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs- und Affektpsychologie. IX. Erfolg und Misserfolg [Studies of the psychology of action and affect. IX. Success and failure]. Psychologische Forschung, 14, 1–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hörhold, M., & Walschburger, P. (1997). Depressive Störung als Ausdruck misslingender Handlungskontrolle: Überprüfung einer psychophysiologischen Belastungsdiagnostik [Depressive disorders as an expression of failed action control: Testing a psychophysiological assessment]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie: Forschung und Praxis, 26, 31–27.Google Scholar
Hormuth, S. E. (1986). The sampling of experience in situ. Journal of Personality, 54, 262–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horner, M. S. (1974a). Performance of men in noncompetitive and interpersonal competitive achievement-oriented situations. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 237–254). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Horner, M. S. (1974b). The measurement and behavioral implications of fear of succes in women. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 91–117). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Hosenfeld, B. (1988). Persönlichkeitsveränderungen im Erwachsenenalter aus der Sicht Jugendlicher [Personality changes in adulthood from the perspective of adolescents]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Free University of Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Housten, D. M. (1995). Vulnerability to depressive mood reactions: Retesting the hoplessness model of depression. British Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 293–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hovland, C. I., & Sears, R. R. (1938). Experiments on motor conflict: I. Types of conflict and their modes of resolution. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 477–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudley, C., & Graham, S. (1993). An attributional intervention to reduce peer directed aggression among African-American boys. Child Development, 64, 124–138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hull, C. L. (1930). Knowledge and purpose as habit mechanisms. Psychological Review, 37, 511–525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1932). The goal and maze learning. Psychological Review, 39, 25–434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1933). Differential habituation to internal stimuli in the albino rat. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 16, 255–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1934). The concept of the habit-family hierarchy, and maze learning. Psychological Review, 41, 33–54; 134–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1952). A behavior system: An introduction to behavior theory concerning the individual organism. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Hundertmark, J. (1990). Entwicklungsbezogene Intentionen im Lebenslauf: Selbstbild und normative Entwicklungsvorstellungen als Einflußfaktoren [Development-related intentions across the lifespan: The role of self-related and normative conceptions]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, Technical University of Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Hundertmark, J., & Heckhausen, J. (1994). Entwicklungsziele junger, mittelalter und alter Erwachsener [Developmental goals in young, middle, and old adulthood]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 26, 197–217.Google Scholar
Hunt, J. M. V. (1965). Intrinsic motivation and its role in psychological development. In Levine, D. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 13 (pp. 189–282). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Hyland, M. E., Curtis, C., & Mason, D. (1985). Fear of success: Motive and cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1669–1677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ikemoto, S., & Panksepp, J. (1999). The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: A unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Research Reviews, 31(1), 6–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T. R. (1997). A neurobiological basis of social attachment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(6), 726–735.Google ScholarPubMed
Insel, T. R., Winslow, J. T., Wang, Z., & Young, L. J. (1998). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neuroendocrine basis of pair bond formation. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, 449, 215–224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irani, B. G., & Haskell-Luevano, C. (2005). Feeding effects of melanocortin ligands – A historical perspective. Peptides, 26(10), 1788–1799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irle, M., & Krolage, J. (1973). Kognitive Konsequenzen irrtümlicher Selbsteinschätzung [Cognitive implications of erroneous self-evaluations]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 4, 36–50.Google Scholar
Irwin, F. W. (1953). Stated expectations as functions of probability and desirability of outcomes. Journal of Personality, 21, 329–339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irwin, F. W. (1971). Intentional behavior and motivation: A cognitive theory. Philadelphia: Lippincott.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion.New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, D. N. (1974). Manual for the Personality Research Form. Goshen, NY: Research Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Jackson, L. A., & Larrance, D. T. (1979). Is a “refinement” of attribution theory necessary to accommodate the learned helplessness reformulation? A critique of the reformulation of Abramson, Seligman & Teasdale. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 681–682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: The flow state scale 2 and dispositional flow scale 2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24, 133–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, B. (1958). A method for investigating the cue characteristics of pictures. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 617–629). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Jagacinski, C. M., & Nicholls, J. G. (1987). Competence and affect in task involvement and ego involvement: The impact of social comparison information. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 107–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jagacinski, C. M., & Nicholls, J. G. (1990). Reducing effort to protect perceived ability: They'd do it but I wouldn't. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 15–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (2 vols).New York: Holt.Google Scholar
James, W. (1892). Psychology: The briefer course. New York: Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janke, W., Erdmann, G., & Kallus, W. (1985). Stressverarbeitungsfragebogen (SVF) [Stress management questionnaire (SVF)]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Janos, O., & Papoušek, H. (1977). Acquisition of appetition and palpebral conditioned reflexes by the same infants. Early Human Development, 1, 91–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaudas, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2004, April). Führen Vorsätze zu Rigidität im Zielstreben? [Do implementation intentions lead to rigidity in goal pursuit?] Paper presented at the symposium “Recent developments in research on implementation intentions” at the 46th meeting of the Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen (TeaP), Giessen, Germany.Google Scholar
Jemmott, J. B. III (1982). Psychosocial stress, social motives and disease susceptibility. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Jemmott, J. B. (1987). Social motives and susceptibility to disease: Stalking individual differences in health risks. Journal of Personality, 55, 267–298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jemmott, J. B., Borysenko, J. Z., Borysenko, M., McClelland, D. C., Chapman, R., Meyer, D., & Benson, H. (1983). Academic stress, power motivation, and decrease in salivary secretory immunoglobulin A secretion rate. Lancet, 1400–1402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jemmott, J. B., & Locke, S. E. (1984). Psychosocial factors, immunologic mediation, and human susceptibility to infections diseases: How much do we know?Psychological Bulletin, 95, 78–108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkins, S. R. (1994). Need for achievement and women's careers over 14 years: Evidence for occupational structure effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 922–932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, T. N., Warner, L. H., & Warden, C. J. (1926). Standard apparatus for the study of animal behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 6, 361–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jennings, K. D. (1991). Early development of mastery motivation and its relation to the self-concept. In Bullock, M. (Ed.), The development of intentional action: Cognitive, motivational and interactive processes. Contributions to human development (pp. 1–13). Basel, Switzerland: Karger.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E. (1976). How do people perceive the causes of behavior?American Scientist, 64, 237–246.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 219–266). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E., & McGillis, D. (1976). Correspondent inferences and the attribution cube: A comparative appraisal. In Harvey, J. H., Ickes, W. J., & Kidd, R. F. (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (Vol. 1, pp. 387–420). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. New York: General Learning.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E., Worchel, S., Goethals, G. R., & Grumet, J. F. (1971). Prior expectancy and behavioral extremity as determinants of attitude attribution. Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 7, 59–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, E. E., Rock, L., Shaver, K. G., Goethals, G. R., & Ward, L. M. (1968). Pattern of performance and ability attribution: An unexpected primacy effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 317–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, H. E., & Jones, M. C. (1928). A study of fear. Childhood Education, 5, 136–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, S. C. (1973). Self- and interpersonal evaluations: Esteem theories versus consistency theories. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 185–199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jopt, U.-J. (1974). Extrinsische Motivation und Leistungsverhalten [Extrinsic motivation and achievement behavior]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Joule, R. V., & Beauvois, J.-L. (1998). Cognitive dissonance theory: A radical view. European Review of Social Psychology, 8, 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jucknat, M. (1938). Leistung, Anspruchsniveau und Selbstbewußtsein [Achievement, level of aspiration, and self-confidence]. Psychologische Forschung, 22, 89–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jung, C. G. (1936/1990). Typologie [Typology]. Munich, Germany: dtv.Google Scholar
Jungermann, H., Pfister, H.-R., & May, R. S. (1994). Competing motivations or changing choices: Conjectures and some data on choice-action consistency. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 195–208). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Junker, E. (1960). Über unterschiedliches Behalten eigener Leistungen[On the differing retention of one's own achievement]. Frankfurt, Germany: Waldemar Kramer.Google Scholar
Kafka, F. (1986). Briefe an Milena [Letters to Milena]. Frankfurt am Main: Fischer.Google Scholar
Kästele, G. (1988). Anlage- und umweltbedingte Determinanten der Handlungs- und Lageorientierung nach Misserfolg im Vergleich zu anderen Persönlichkeitseigenschaften [Genetic and environmental determinants of failure-related action and state orientation in compari son with other personality traits]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Osnabrück, Germany.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1988). The meanings of personality predicates. American Psychologist, 43, 614–620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, S. (1981). Ecology and the acculturation of cognitive and social styles among Mexican American children. Hispanic Journal of Behavior Sciences, 3(2), 111–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). On the interpretation of intuitive probability: A reply to Jonathan Cohen. Cognition, 7, 409–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39, 341–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kammer, D. (1983). Eine Untersuchung der psychometrischen Eigenschaften des deutschen Beck-Depressionsinventars (BDI) [An investigation of the psychometric properties of the German Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)]. Diagnostica, 29, 48–60.Google Scholar
Kammer, D., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1987). Depressive Attribuierung: Eine Standortbestimmung [Depressive attribution: The state of the art]. In Ameland, M. (Ed.), Bericht über den 35. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie in Heidelberg (Vol. 2, pp. 557–566). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kanfer, R. (1990). Motivation theory and industrial and organizational psychology. In Dunnette, M. D. & Hough, L. M. (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 75–170). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Karabenick, J. D., & Heller, K. A. (1976). A developmental study of effort and ability attributions. Developmental Psychology, 12, 559–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karabenick, S. A. (1972). Valence of success and failure as a function of achievement motives and locus of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 101–110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karabenick, S. A., & Yousseff, Z. I. (1968). Performance as a function of achievement level and perceived difficulty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 414–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karasawa, M., Little, T. D., Miyashita, T., Mashima, M., & Azuma, H. (1997). Japanese children's action-control beliefs about school performance. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 405–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karau, S. J., & Williams, K. D. (1993). Social loafing: A meta-analytic review and theoretical integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 681–706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufmann, I. C., & Rosenblum, L. A. (1969). The reaction of separation from mother on the emotional behavior of infant monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 159, 681–695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K. (1977a). Thickening thin data: The maternal role in developing communication and language. In Bullowa, M. (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 191–206). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. (1977b). Toward the origin of dialogue. In Schaffer, H. R. (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp. 89–117). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kazén, M., Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Self-infiltration vs. self-compatibility checking in dealing with unattractive tasks and unpleasant items: The moderating influence of state vs. action orientation. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 157–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kazén, M., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Intention memory and achievement motivation: Volitional facilitation and inhibition as a function of affective contents of need-related stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 426–448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keenan, J. P., Nelson, A., Connor, O' M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Self-recognition and the right hemisphere. Nature, 409, 305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keeney, R. L., & Reiffa, H. (1976). Decisions with multiple objectives: Preferences and value tradeoffs. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kehr, H. M. (2004a). Implicit(explicit motive discrepancies and volitional depletion among managers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 315–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehr, H. M. (2004b). Integrating implicit motives, explicit motives, and perceived abilities: The compensatory model of work motivation and volition. Academy of Management Review, 29, 479–499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H. (1997a). Entwicklungspsychopathologie: Das Entstehen von Verhaltensproblemen in der frühesten Kindheit. [Develop mental psychopathology: The emergence of behavioral problems in infancy]. In Keller, H. (Ed.), Handbuch der Kleinkindforschung (pp. 625–641). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Keller, H. (1997b). Kontinuität und Entwicklung [Continuity and development]. In Keller, H. (Ed.), Handbuch der Kleinkindforschung. (pp. 235–258). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Keller, H. (1997c). Evolutionary approaches. In Berry, J., Poortinga, Y., & Panndey, J. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, theory and method (Vol. 1, pp. 215–255). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Keller, H. (2000). Human parent-child relationships from an evolutionary perspective. American Behavioral Scientist, 43, 957–969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., & Gauda, G. (1987). Eye contact in the first months of life and its developmental consequences. In Rauh, H. & Steinhausen, H.-C. (Eds.), Psychobiology and early development (advances in psychology) (pp. 129–143). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Keller, H., Lohaus, A., Völker, S., Cappenberg, M., & Chasiotis, A. (1999). Temporal contingency as a measure of interactional quality. Child Development, 70, 474–485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., Lohaus, A., Völker, S., Elben, C., & Ball, J. (2003). Warmth and contingency and their relationship to maternal attitudes toward parenting. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164, 275–292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In Levine, D. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 192–238). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Kelley, H. H. (1971). Attribution in social interaction. New York: General Learning.Google Scholar
Kelley, H. H. (1972). Causal schemata and the attribution process. New York: General Learning.Google Scholar
Kelley, H. H. (1973). The process of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, H. H., & Michela, J. L. (1980). Attribution theory and research. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 457–501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, G. (1962). Comments on J. Brehm “Motivational effects of cognitive dissonance.” In Jones, M. P. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 78–81). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keltner, D., & Robinson, R. J. (1996). Extremism, power, and the imagined basis of social conflict. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 101–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keltner, D., & Robinson, R. J. (1997). Defending the status quo: Power and bias in social conflict. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1066–1077.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemmler, L. (1957). Untersuchungen über den frühkindlichen Trotz [Studies on defiance in early childhood]. Psychologische Forschung, 25, 279–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenrick, D. T., Li, N. P., & Butner, J. (2003). Dynamical evolutionary psychology: Individual decision rules and emergent social norms. Psychological Review, 110, 3–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendrick, K. M. (2004). The neurobiology of social bonds. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 16(12), 1007–1008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keverne, E. B., & Curley, J. P. (2004). Vasopressin, oxytocin and social behaviour. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(6), 777–783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keverne, E. B., & Kendrick, K. M. (1994). Maternal behaviour in sheep and its neuroendocrine regulation. Acta Paediatrica, 397(Suppl), 47–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keverne, E. B., Martensz, N. D., & Tuite, B. (1989). Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 14(1–2), 155–161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killcross, S., Robbins, T. W., & Everitt, B. J. (1997). Different types of fear-conditioned behaviour mediated by separate nuclei within amygdala. Nature, 388(6640), 377–380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, J. E., & Figueredo, A. J. (1997). The five-factor model plus dominance in chimpanzee personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 257–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, L. A. (1995). Wishes, motives, goals, and personal memories: Relations of measures of human motivation. Journal of Personality, 63, 985–1007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kipnis, D. (1972). Does power corrupt?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 33–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kipnis, D. (1974). The powerholder. In Tedeschi, J. T. (Ed.), Perspectives on social power (pp. 82–122). Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
Kipnis, D. (1976). The powerholders. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kircher, T. T. J., Brammer, M., Bullmore, E., Simmons, A., Bartels, M., & David, A. S. (2002). The neural correlates of intentional and incidental self processing. Neuropsychologia, 40, 683–692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirschbaum, C., Wolf, O., Wippich, W., & Hellhammer, D. (1996). Stress- and treatment-induced elevations of cortisol levels associated with impaired declarative memory in healthy adults. Life Science, 58, 1475–1483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klandermans, P. G. (1983). Rotter's I-E-scale and socio-political action-taking: The balance of 20 years of research. European Journal of Social Psychology, 13, 399–415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, S. B., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Chance, S. (2002). Decisions and the evolution of memory: Multiple systems, multiple functions. Psychological Review, 109, 306–329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinbeck, U. (1975). Motivation und Berufswahl [Motivation and occupational choice]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kleinbeck, U. (1996). Arbeitsmotivation: Entstehung, Wirkung und Förderung [Work motivation: Origins, effects, and promotion]. Weinheim, Germany: Juventa.Google Scholar
Klineberg, D. (1938). Emotional expression in Chinese literature. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 33, 517–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klinger, E. (1967). Modelling effects on achievement imagery. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 49–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klinger, E. (1971). Structure and functions of fantasy. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review, 82, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and the incentives in people's lives. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Klüver, H., & Bucy, P. C. (1937). “Psychic blindness” and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobectomy in rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Physiology, 119, 352–353.Google Scholar
Klüver, H., & Bucy, P. C. (1939). Preliminary analysis of functions of the temporal lobes in monkeys. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 42, 979–1000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koch, S. (1956). Behavior as “intrinsically” regulated: Work notes towards a pre-theory of phenomena called “motivational.” In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 42–87). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Koch, S. (Ed.) (1959–1963). Psychology: A study of science. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 1091–1111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kock, S. E. (1965). Företagsledning och motivation. Svenska Handeinshögskolan, Affärsekonomiska Förlagsförenigen, Helsingfors.Google Scholar
Kock, S. E. (1974). Företagsledning och motivation. Nordisk Psykologi, 26, 211–219.Google Scholar
Koepp, M. J., Gunn, R. N., Lawrence, A. D., Cunningham, V. J., Dagher, A., Jones, T., Brooks, D. J., Bench, C. J., & Grasby, P. M. (1998). Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature, 393(6682), 266–268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koestner, R., & McClelland, D. C. (1990). Perspectives on competence motivation. In Pervin, L. (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 527–548). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Koestner, R., Weinberger, J., & McClelland, D. C. (1991). Task-intrinsic and social-extrinsic sources of arousal for motives assessed in fantasy and self-report. Journal of Personality, 59, 57–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koestner, R., Zuckerman, M., & Koestner, J. (1987). Praise, involvement and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(2), 383–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohli, M. (1981). Zur Theorie der biographischen Selbst- und Fremdthematisierung [Theory of biographical accounts of the self and others]. In Matthes, J. (Ed.), Lebenswelt und soziale Probleme: Verhandlungen des 20. Deutschen Soziologentages (pp. 502–520). Frankfurt, Germany: Campus.Google Scholar
Kohut, H. (1979). Die Heilung des Selbst [Healing the self]. Frankfurt, Germany: Suhrkamp.Google Scholar
Köller, O. (2000). Leistungsgruppierung, soziale Vergleiche und selbstbezogene Fähigkeitskognitionen in der Schule [Ability grouping, social comparisons, and self-related ability cognitions in school]. Habilitationsschrift, Univeristy of Potsdam, Germany.Google Scholar
Köller, O., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., & Baumert, J. (2006). Zum Zusammenspiel von schulischer Leistung, Selbstkonzept und Interesse in der gymnasialen Oberstufe [On the interplay of academic achievement, self-concept, and interest in upper secondary schools]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 20, 27–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konner, M. J. (1981). Evolution of human behavioral development. In Monroe, R. H., Monroe, R. L., & Whiting, B. B. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural human development (pp. 3–51). New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Koole, S. L. (2000). Positivity in self-evaluation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Free University of Amsterdam, Netherlands.Google Scholar
Koole, S. L. (2004). Volitional shielding of the self: Effects of action orientation and external demand on implicit self-evaluation. Social Cognition, 22, 117–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koole, S. L., & Jostmann, N. (2004). Getting a grip on your feelings: Effects of action orientation and social demand on intuitive affect regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(6), 974–989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koole, S. L., Smeets, K., Knippenberg, A., & Dijksterhuis, A. (1999). The cessation of rumination through self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 111–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kornadt, H.-J. (1982a). Band 1: Empirische u. theoretische Untersuchungen zu einer Motivationstheorie der Aggression und zur Konstruktvalidierung eines Aggressions-TAT [Volume 1: Empirical and theoretical studies on a motivational theory of aggression and on the construct validation of an aggression TAT]. Aggressionsmotiv und Aggressionshemmung.Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Kornadt, H.-J. (1982b). Grundzüge einer Motivationstheorie der Aggression [Outline of a motivational theory of aggression]. In Hilke, R. & Kempf, W. (Eds.), Aggression (pp. 86–111). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Kornadt, H.-J., Eckensberger, L. H., & Emminghaus, W. B. (1980). Cross-cultural research on motivation and its contribution to a general theory of motivation. In Triandis, H. C. (Ed.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology (Vol. 3: Basic processes) (pp. 223–321). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435(7042), 673–676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krampen, G. (1987). Differential effects of teacher comments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 137–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krantz, D. L., & Allen, D. (1967). The rise and fall of McDougall's instinct doctrine. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 3, 326–338.3.0.CO;2-6>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krapp, A. (1999). Intrinsische Lernmotivation und Interesse: Forschungsansätze und konzeptuelle Überlegungen [Intrinsic learning motivation and interest: Research approaches and conceptual considerations]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 45, 387–406.Google Scholar
Krapp, A. (2001). Interesse [Interest]. In Rost, D. H. (Ed.), Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie (2nd ed., pp. 286–293). Weinheim, Germany: PVU.Google Scholar
Krapp, A., Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. (1992). Interest, learning and development. In Renninger, K. A. & Hidi, S. (Eds.), The role of interest and development(pp. 3–25). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Krau, E. (1982). Motivational feedback loops in the structure of action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1030–1040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, J. R. (1980). Optimal foraging, predation risk and territory defense. Area, 68, 83–90.Google Scholar
Krech, D., Crutchfield, R. S., & Ballachey, E. L. (1962). Individual in society.New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Krohne, H. W. (1988). Erziehungsstil forschung: Neuere theoretische Ansätze und empirische Befunde [Child-rearing research: New theoretical approaches and empirical results]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 2, 157–172.Google Scholar
Krohne, H. W. (1996). Angst und Angstbewältigung [Anxiety and coping with anxiety]. Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Krug, S. (1976). Förderung und Änderung des Leistungsmotivs: Theoretische Grundlagen und deren Anwendung [Fostering and modifying the achievement motive: Theoretical fundamentals and their application]. In Schmalt, H.-D. & Meyer, W.-U. (Eds.), Leistungsmotivation und Verhalten (pp. 221–247). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett.Google Scholar
Krüger, H. (1978). Anfänge der Entwicklung des Anstrengungskonzepts im Kindergartenalter [Early development of the effort concept at kindergarten age]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Krüger, J., & Heckhausen, J. (1993). Personality development across the adult life span: Subjective conceptions versus cross-sectional contrasts. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 48, 100–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krüger, J., Heckhausen, J., & Hundertmark, J. (1995). Perceiving middle aged adults: Effects of stereotype-congruent and incongruent information. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 50B, 82–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cognitive and motivational bases. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.). (1996). Social psychology: A handbook of basic principles (pp. 329–360). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kubinger, K. D., & Ebenhöh, H. (1996). Arbeitshaltungen (AHA): Objektiver Persönlichkeitstest [Work styles: Objective personality test]. Mödling, Austria: Schuhfried.Google Scholar
Kubinger, K. D., & Litzenberger, M. (2003). Zur Validität der Objektiven Persönlichkeits-Test-Batterie “Arbeitshaltungen” [On the validity of the objective personality test battery “Work Style”]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 24, 119–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Külpe, O. (1893). Grundriß der Psychologie. Auf experimenteller Grundlage dargestellt [Outlines of Psychology. Based upon the Results of Experimental Investigation]. Leipzig, Germany: Wilhelm Engelmann.Google Scholar
Kuester, J., & Paul, A. (1989). Reproductive strategies of subadult barbary macaque males at Affenberg Salem. In Rasa, A. E., Vogel, C., & Voland, E. (Eds.), The sociobiology of sexual and reproductive strategies (pp. 93–109). London: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1977). Meß- und prozeßtheoretische Analysen einiger Person- und Situationsparameter der Leistungsmotivation[Personal and situational determinants of achievement motivation: Computer simulation and experimental analysis]. Bonn, Germany: Bouvier.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1978a). Situations-, reaktions- und personbezogene Konsistenz des Leistungsmotivs bei der Messung mittels des Heckhausen TAT [Situation-, reaction-, and person-related consistency of the achievement motive measured using Heckhausen's TAT]. Archiv für Psychologie, 130, 37–52.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1978b). Standard setting and risk preference: An elaboration of the theory of achievement motivation and an empirical test. Psychological Review, 85, 239–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1981). Motivational and functional helplessness: The moderating effect of state vs. action orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 155–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1982). The expectancy-value approach in the theory of social motivation. In Feather, N. T. (Ed.), Expectations and actions: Expectancy-value models in psychology (pp. 125–162). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1983). Motivation, Konflikt und Handlungskontrolle [Motivation, conflict, and action control]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1984). Motivational aspects of achievement motivation and learned helplessness: Toward a comprehensive theory of action control. In Maher, B. A. & Maher, W. B. (Eds.), Progress in experimental personality research (Vol. 13, pp. 99–171). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1987). Action control: The maintenance of motivational states. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 279–291). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1994). Action versus state orientation: Psychometric properties of the Action-Control-Scale (ACS-90). In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Action control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 47–59). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1996). Wille und Freiheitserleben: Formen der Selbststeuerung [The will and perceived freedom: Forms of volition]. In Kuhl, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Motivation, Volition und Handlung(Series IV, Vol. 4, pp. 665–765). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1998). Wille und Persönlichkeit: Von der Funktionsanalyse zur Aktivierungsdynamik psychischer Systeme [Volition and personality: Functional analysis of self-regulation]. Psychologische Rundschau, 49, 61–77.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (2000a). A functional-design approach to motivation and volition: The dynamics of personality systems interactions. In Boekaerts, M., Pintrich, P. R., & Zeidner, M. (Eds.), Self-regulation: Directions and challenges for future research (pp. 111–169). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (2000b). A theory of self-development: Affective fixation and the STAR model of personality disorders and related styles. In Heckhausen, J. (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 187–211). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (2000c). The volitional basis of personality systems interaction theory: Applications in learning and treatment contexts. International Journal of Educational Research, 33, 665–703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (2001). Motivation und Persönlichkeit: Die Interaktion psychischer Systeme [Motivation and personality: The interaction of psychological systems]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Baumann, N. (2000). Self-regulation and rumination: Negative affect and impaired self-accessibility. In Perrig, W. & Grob, A. (Eds.), Control of human behavior mental processes and consciousness: Essays in honor of the 60th birthday of August Flammer (pp. 283–305). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1983). Handlungskontrolle und Umfang der Informa tionsverarbeitung: Wahl einer einfachen (nicht optimalen) Entscheidungsregel zug unsten rascher Handlungsbereitschaft [Action control and scope of information processing: Selection of a simplified (non-optimal) decision rule to achieve fast readiness for action]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 14, 241–250.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1985). Action control theory: From cognition to behavior. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1994a). Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1994b). Alienation: Ignoring one's preferences. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Blankenship, V. (1979). The dynamic theory of achievement motivation: From episodic to dynamic thinking. Psychological Review, 86, 141–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Fuhrmann, A. (1998). Decomposing self-regulation and self-control: The volitional components checklist. In Heckhausen, J. & Dweck, C. (Eds.), Life span perspectives on motivation and control (pp. 15–49). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Goschke, T. (1994). State orientation and the activation and retrieval of intentions from memory. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 127–152). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Helle, P. (1986). Motivational and volitional determinants of depression: The degenerated intention hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 247–251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, J., & Kaschel, R. (2004). Entfremdung als Krankheitsursache: Selbstregulation von Affekten und integrative Kompetenz [Alienation as a determinant of symptom formation: Self-regulation of affect and integrative competence]. Psychologische Rundschau, 55, 61–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (1994). Self-discrimination and memory: State orientation and false ascription of assigned activities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 1103–1115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (1999). Volitional facilitation of difficult intentions: Joint activation of intention memory and positive affect removes Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 382–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (2003). Handlungs- und Lageorientierung: Wie lernt man, seine Gefühle zu steuern? [Action and state orientation: How do people learn to control their feelings?] In Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. & Rheinberg, F. (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (2004). Impress them or convince them? Sales performance, social needs and psychological well-being as a function of histrionic vs. action oriented personality. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Kuhl, J., & Scheffer, D. (1999). Der operante Multi-Motiv-Test (OMT): Manual [The operant Multi-Motive Test (OMT): Manual]. University of Osnabrück, Germany.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Völker, S. (1998). Entwicklung und Persönlichkeit [Development and personality]. In Keller, H. (Ed.), Lehrbuch der Entwicklungspsychologie (pp. 207–240). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Weiß, M. (1994). Performance deficits following uncontrollable failure: Impaired action control or global attributions and generalized expectancy deficits? In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation.Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kukla, A. (1972a). Attributional determinants of achievement-related behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 166–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kukla, A. (1972b). Foundations of an attributional theory of performance. Psychological Review, 79, 454–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kukla, A. (1978). An attributional theory of choice. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 113–144). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kummer, H. (1995). Causal knowledge in animals. In Sperber, D., Premack, D., & Premack, A. James (Eds.), A causal cognition (pp. 26–39). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Kun, A. (1977). Evidence for preschoolers' understanding of causal direction in extended causal sequences. Child Development, 49, 218–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kun, A., Parsons, J. E., & Ruble, D. (1974). The development of integration processes using ability and effort information to predict outcome. Developmental Psychology, 10 (5), 721–732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kun, A., & Weiner, B. (1973). Necessary versus sufficient causal schemata for success and failure. Journal of Research in Personality, 7, 197–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunda, Z. (1999). Social cognition: Making sense of people. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kunde, W., Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation and execution of actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 57A, 87–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lacey, J. I. (1969). Somatic response patterning and stress: Some revisions of activation theory. In Appley, M. H. & Trumbull, R. (Eds.), Psychological stress: Issues and research (pp. 14–39). New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
Lang, F. R. (2001). Regulation of social relationship in later adulthood. Journal of Gerontology, Psychological Sciences, 56B, 321–329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, F. R. (2004). Social motivation across the life span. In Lang, F. R. & Fingerman, K. L. (Eds.), Growing together: Personal relationships across the lifespan (pp. 341–367). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lang, F. R., & Heckhausen, J. (2001). Perceived control over development and subjective well-being: Differential benefits across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 509–523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, F. R., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). Stabilisierung und Kontinuität der Persönlichkeit im Lebensverlauf [Stabilization and continuity of personality across the lifespan]. In Asendorpf, J. B. (Ed.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie (Vol. C/V/3). Soziale, emotionale und Persönlichkietsentwicklung (pp. 525–562). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Lang, F. R., & Heckhausen, J. (2006). Developmental changes of motivation and interpersonal capacities across adulthood: Managing the challenges and constraints of social contexts. In Hoare, C. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of adult development and learning (pp. 149–166). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lang, F. R., Reschke, F. S., & Neyer, F. J. (2006). Social relationships, transitions and personality development across the life span. In Mroczek, D. K. & Little, T. D. (Eds.), Handbook of personality development (pp. 445–466). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. H. (1999). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. Gainesville: NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, University of Florida. (this reference to Lang et al. replaces the one to CEA, please substitute in master reference list)Google Scholar
Lange, L. (1888). Neue Experimente über den Vorgang der einfachen Reaktion auf Sinneseindrücke [New experiments on the process of simple reaction to sense impressions]. Philosophische Studien, 4, 479–510.Google Scholar
Langens, T. A. (2001). Predicting behavior change in Indian businessmen from a combination of need for achievement and self-discrepancy. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 339–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langens, T. A. (2002). Tagträume, Anliegen und Motivation [Daydreams, personal goals, and motivation]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Lansing, J. B., & Heyns, R. W. (1959). Need affiliation and frequency of four types of communication. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 365–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latané, B., Williams, K., & Harkins, S. (1979). Many hands make light the work: The causes and consequences of social loafing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 822–832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawler, E. E. (1968). A correlational-causal analysis of the relationship between expectancy attitudes and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 52, 462–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawler, E. E., & Porter, L. W. (1967). Antecedent attitudes of effective managerial job performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 2, 122–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, D. M., & Festinger, L. (1962). Deterrents and reinforcement: The psychology of insufficient reward. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Lawrence, P. R., & Nohria, N. (2002). Driven: How human nature shapes our choices. San Francisco: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. A., & Launier, R. (1979). Stress-related transactions between person and environment. In Pervin, L. A. & Lewis, M. (Eds.), Perspectives in interactional psychology (pp. 287–372). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1968). Emotion and adaption: Conceptual and empirical relations. In Arnold, W. J. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 175–270). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. American Psychologist, 39, 124–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarus, R. S., Opton, E. M., Nomikos, M. S., & Rankin, N. D. (1965). The principle of short-circuiting of threat: Further evidence. Journal of Personality, 33, 622–635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnen, J., & Tallon, S. (1966). La periodicité spontanée de la prise d'aliments ad libitum du rat blanc. Journal de Physiologie, 58, 323–349.Google Scholar
Lecci, L., Karoly, P., Briggs, C., & Kuhn, K. (1994). Specificity and generality of motivational components in depression: A personal projects analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 404–408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. (1995). Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 209–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Touchstone.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (2002). The synaptic self. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Lee, F. K., Sheldon, K. M., & Turban, D. B. (2003). Personality and the goal-striving process: The influence of achievement goal patterns, goal level and mental focus on performance and enjoyment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 256–265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leeper, R. W. (1935). The role of motivation in learning: A study of the phenomenon of differential motivational control of the utilization of habits. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 4b, 3–40.Google Scholar
Lefcourt, H. M. (1976). Locus of control: Current trends in theory and research.New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lehmann, H. C., & Witty, P. A. (1934). Faculty psychology and personality traits. American Journal of Psychology, 46, 486–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leibowitz, S. F., Weiss, G. F., Walsh, U. A., & Viswanath, D. (1989). Medial hypothalamic serotonin: Role in circadian patterns of feeding and macronutritient selection. Brain Research, 503, 132–140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Reflective and reflexive action control in patients with frontal brain lesions. Neuropsychology, 15, 80–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leon-Villagra, J., Meyer, W. U., & Engler, U. (1990). Ability judgments based on praise: A developmental psychology study. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 22 (1), 54–74.Google Scholar
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the overjustification hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, R. M. (2002). Concepts and theories of human development (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lerner, R. M., & Busch-Rossnagel, N. A. (1981). Individuals as producers of their development: A life-span perspective.New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lersch, P. (1938). Aufbau des Charakters [Structure of the character]. Leipzig, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
Lersch, P. (1951). Aufbau der Person [Structure of the person] (4th ed. of Aufbau des Charakters). Munich, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
LeVay, S., & Hamer, D. H. (1994). Evidence for a biological influence in male homosexuality. Scientific American, May, 44–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leventahl, H. (1984). A perceptual-motor theory of emotion. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 17, pp. 117–182). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Levesque, J., Fanny, E., Joanett, Y., Paquette, V., Mensour, B., Beaudouin, G., Leroux, J.-M., Borugouin, P., & Beauregard, M. (2003). Neural circuitry underlying voluntary suppression of sadness. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 502–510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, A. S., & Billington, C. J. (1997). Why do we eat? A neural systems approach. Annual Review of Nutrition, 17, 597–619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, A. S., & Billington, C. J. (2004). Opioids as agents of reward-related feeding: A consideration of the evidence. Physiology & Behavior, 82 (1), 57–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, A. S., Kotz, C. M., & Gosnell, B. A. (2003). Sugars and fats: The neurobiology of preference. Journal of Nutrition, 133 (3), 831S–834S.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1922). Das Problem der Willensmessung und das Grundgesetz der Assoziation. II [The problem of measuring volition and the law of association. II]. Psychologische Forschung, 2, 65–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1926a). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs- und Affekt-Psychologie I.: Vorbemerkungen über die psychischen Kräfte und Energien und über die Struktur der Seele [Studies of the psychology of action and affect. I: Preliminary remarks on the psychic powers and energies and the structure of the soul]. Psychologische Forschung, 7, 294–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1926b). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs- und Affekt-Psychologie II.: Vorsatz, Wille und Bedürfnis [Studies of the psychology of action and affect. II: Intent, volition, and need]. Psychologische Forschung, 7, 330–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1931a). Die psychologische Situation bei Lohn und Strafe [The psychological elements in rewards and punishment]. Leipzig, Germany: Hirzel.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1931b). Environmental forces in child behavior and development. In Murchison, C. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 94–127). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1932). Ersatzhandlung und Ersatzbefriedigung [Substitute action and substitute satisfaction]. Bericht über den 12. Kongreß der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie, Hamburg, 1931 (pp. 382–384). Jena, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1934). Der Richtungsbegriff in der Psychologie: Der spezielle und allgemeine hodologische Raum [The concept of direction in psychology: Special and general hodological space]. Psychologische Forschung, 19, 249–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality: Selected papers. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of topological psychology.New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1938). The conceptual representation and the measurement of psychological forces. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1939). Field theory and experiment in social psychology. American Journal of Sociology, 44, 868–897.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1942). Field theory of learning. Yearbook of National Social Studies of Education, 41, 215–242.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1943). Defining the “field at a given time.”Psychological Review, 50, 292–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1946a). Action research and minority problems. Journal of Social Issues, 2, 34–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1946b). Behavior and development as a function of the total situation. In Carmichael, L. (Ed.), Manual of child psychology (pp. 791–844). New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1947). Group decision and social change. In Maccoby, E. E., Newcomb, T. M., & Hartley, E. L. (Eds.), Readings in social psychology (pp. 197–211). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1963). Feldtheorie in den Sozialwissenschaften [Field theory in the social sciences]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Lewin, K., Dembo, T., Festinger, L., & Sears, P. S. (1944). Level of aspiration. In McHunt, J. (Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders (Vol. 1, pp. 333–378). New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Toward a theory of social cognition: The development of the self. In Social interaction and communication during infancy: New directions for child developmen (pp. 1–20). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., & Goldberg, S. (1969). Perceptual-cognitive development in infancy: A generalized expectancy model as a function of the mother-infant interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 15, 81–100.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2002). Cortisol response to embarrassment and shame. Child Development, 73, 1034–1045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, N. P., Bailey, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., & Linsenmeier, J. A. W. (2002). The necessities and luxuries of mate preference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 947–955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 529–566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, M. D. (2003). Reflective and reflexive judgment processes: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. In Forgas, J. P., Williams, K. R., & Hippel, W. V. (Eds.), Social judgments: Implicit and explicit processes (pp. 44–67). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Liebert, R. M., & Morris, L. W. (1967). Cognitive and emotional components of text anxiety: A distinction and some initial data. Psychological Reports, 20, 975–978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lienert, G. (1969). Testaufbau und Testanalyse [Test design and test analysis]. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Lindsley, D. B. (1957). Psychophysiology and motivation. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1957 (pp. 44–105). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Lindworsky, J. (1923). Der Wille: Seine Erscheinung und seine Beherrschung [The will: Its emergence and its mastery] (3rd ed.). Leipzig, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2003). The role of self-efficacy beliefs in student engagement and learning in the classroom. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 19, 119–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linville, P. W. (1987). Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer against stress-related illness and depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 663–676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lippitt, R. (1940). An experimental study of the effect of democratic and authoritarian group atmospheres. University of Iowa Studies in Child Welfare, 16, 45–195.Google Scholar
Lissner, K. (1933). Die Entspannung von Bedürfnissen durch Ersatzhandlungen [Relieving needs by substitute actions]. Psychologische Forschung, 18, 218–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, B. R. (1983). Personal projects: A rationale and method for investigation. Environment and Behavior, 15, 273–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, B. R. (1989). Personal projects analysis: Trivial pursuits, magnificent obsessions, and the search for coherence. In Buss, D. M. & Cantor, N. (Eds.), Personality psychology: Recent trends and emerging directions (pp. 15–31). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, B. R. (1999). Personal projects and social ecology: Themes and variations across the life span. In Brandstädter, J. & Lerner, R. (Eds.), Action and self-development: Theory and research through the life span (pp. 197–221). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Little, T. D. (1988). Sociocultural influences on the development of children's action-control beliefs. In Heckhausen, J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulation across the life span (pp. 281–315). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Little, T. D. (1998). Self-regulation and school performance: Is there optimal level of action-control?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70 (1), 54–74.Google Scholar
Little, T. D., & Lopez, D. (1997). Regularities in the development of children's causality beliefs about school performance across six sociocultural contexts. Developmental Psychology, 33, 165–175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, T. D., Lopez, D., Oettingen, G., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). A comparative-longitudinal study of action-control beliefs and school performance: On the role of context. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 237–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, T. D., Oettingen, G., Stetsenko, A., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Children's action-control beliefs about school performance: How do American children compare with German and Russian children?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 686–700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, T. D., Stetsenko, A., & Maier, H. (1999). Action control beliefs and school performance: A longitudinal study of Moscow children and adolescents. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 799–823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Litwin, G. H. (1966). Achievement motivation, expectancy of success, and risk-taking behavior. In Atkinson, J. W. & Feather, N. T. (Eds.), A theory of achievement behavior (pp. 103–115). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Litwin, G. H., & Stringer, R. A. (1968). Motivation and organizational climate. Boston: Harvard University, Graduate School of Business Administration, Division of Research.Google Scholar
Locke, E. A. (1968). Toward a theory of task motivation and incentives. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 157–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, E. A. (1975). Personnel attitudes and motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 26, 457–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation – A 35 year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 705–717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Locke, E. A., & Shaw, K. N. (1984). Atkinson's inverse-U curve and the missing cognitive variables. Psychological Reports, 55, 403–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loehlin, J. C. (1989). Partitioning environmental and genetic contributions to behavioral development. American Psychologist, 44, 1285–1292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, G. D., Schachar, R. J., & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological Science, 8, 60–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez, D. F., Little, T. D., Oettingen, G., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Self regulation and school performance: Is there optimal level of action-control?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 54–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorberbaum, J. P., Newman, J. D., Horwitz, A. R., Dubno, J. R., Lydiard, R. B., Hammer, M. B., Bohning, D. E., & George, M. S. (2002). A potential role for thalamocingulate circuitry in human maternal behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 431–445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenz, K. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. Journal für Ornithologie, 83, 137–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1937). Über die Bildung des Instinktbegriffs [On the construction of the concept of instinct]. Naturwissenschaften, 25, 289–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1943). Die angebotenen Formen möglicher Erfahrung [The forms of potential experience]. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 5, 235–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1950). The comparative method of studying innate behavior patterns. In Society for Experimental Biology (Ed.), Physiological mechanisms in animal behavior (pp. 221–268). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1966). Ethologie, die Biologie des Verhaltens [Ethology, the biology of behavior]. In Gessner, F. & Bertalanffy, L. V. (Eds.), Handbuch der Biologie (Vol. II, pp. 341–559). Frankfurt, Germany: Athenäum.Google Scholar
Lovejoy, C. D. (1981). The origin of man. Science, 211, 341–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowell, E. L. (1950). A methodological study of projectively measured achievement motivation. Unpublished master's thesis, Wesleyan University, Middletown, CT.Google Scholar
Lowell, E. L. (1952). The effect of need for achievement on learning and speed of performance. Journal of Psychology, 33, 31–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowin, A. (1967). Approach and avoidance: Alternate modes of selective exposure to information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luginbuhl, J. E. R., Crowe, D. H., & Kahan, J. P. (1975). Causal attribution for success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 86–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luhmann, N. (1975). Macht [Power]. Stuttgart, Germany: Enke.Google Scholar
Lund, B., Rheinberg, F., & Gladasch, U. (2001). Ein Elterntraining zum motivationsförderlichen Erziehungsverhalten in Leistungskontexten [Teaching parents how to reduce children's fear of failure]. Zeits chrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 15, 130–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lundy, A. (1985). The reliability of the Thematic Apperception Test. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 141–145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundy, A. (1988). Instructional set and Thematic Apperception Test validity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 309–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain: An introduction to neuropsychology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R., & Homskaya, E. D. (1964). Disturbances in the regulative role of speech with frontal lobe lesions. In Warren, J. M. & Akert, K. (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior (pp. 353–371). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Lütkenhaus, P. (1984). Pleasure derived from mastery in three-year olds: Its function for persistence and the influence of maternal behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 7, 343–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lütkenhaus, P., & Bullock, M. (1991). Die Entwicklung von funktionalen und deklarativen Aspekten des Selbstkonzeptes bei Kleinkindern [The development of functional and declarative aspects of the self-concept in toddlers]. Paper presented at the 8th Tagung Entwicklungspsychologie, Berne, Switzerland.Google Scholar
Lütkenhaus, P., Grossman, K. E., & Grossman, K. (1985). Transactional influences of infants' orienting ability and maternal cooperation on competition in three-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 80, 257–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, K. (1988). Social and personality development: An evolutionary synthesis. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
MacDonald, K. (1992). Warmth as a developmental construct: An evolutionary analysis. Child Development, 63, 753–773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machiaovelli, N. (1532). Il principe [The prince] (L. Ricci, Trans.). New York: McGraw Hill).Google Scholar
MacIver, D. J., Stipek, D. J., & Daniels, D. H. (1991). Explaining within-semester changes in student effort in junior high school and senior high school courses. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 201–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., & Jetten, J. (1994). Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 808–817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macrae, C. N., Hewstone, M., & Griffiths, R. J. (1993). Processing load and memory for stereotype-based information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 77–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacTurk, R. H., & Morgan, G. A. (1995). Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptualization and applications. Advances in applied developmental psychology (Vol. 12). Westport, CT: Ablex.Google Scholar
Madsen, K. B. (1959). Theories of motivation.Copenhagen, Denmark: Munksgaard.Google Scholar
Madsen, K. B. (1974). Modern theories of motivation. Copenhagen, Denmark: Munksgaard.Google Scholar
Maehr, M. L. (1974). Culture and achievement motivation. American Psychologist, 29, 887–896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maehr, M. L., & Kleiber, D. (1981). The graying of achievement motivation. American Psychologist, 36, 787–793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magnusson, D., & Endler, N. S. (Eds.). (1977). Personality at the crossroads: Current issues in interactional psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mahler, W. (1933). Ersatzhandlungen verschiedenen Realitätsgrades [Substitute actions on various levels of reality]. Psychologische Forschung, 18, 27–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mai, S. (2004). Erfolg im Verkauf – Entwicklung und Evaluation eines kognitiv-behavioralen Trainings [Success in sales – Development and evaluation of a cognitive behavioral training program]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hildesheim, Germany.Google Scholar
Maier, G. W., & Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Action versus state orientation and disengagement from unrealistic goals. Poster presented at the 107th annual convention of the American Psychological Association, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Major, B. (1980). Information acquisition and attribution processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1010–1023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malmo, R. B. (1959). Activation: A neurophysiological dimension. Psychological Review, 66, 367–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Mandler, G., & Sarason, S. B. (1952). A study of anxiety and learning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 166–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mann, P. E., & Bridges, R. S. (2001). Lactogenic hormone regulation of maternal behavior. Progress in Brain Research, 133, 251–262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mansson, H. H. (1969). The relation of dissonance reduction to cognitive, perceptual, consummatory, and learning measures of thirst. In Zimbardo, P. G. (Ed.), The cognitive control of motivation (pp. 78–97). Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
Marbe, K. (1915). Der Begriff der Bewusstseinslage [The concept of mindset]. Fortschritte der Psychologie und ihrer Anwendungen, 3, 27–39.Google Scholar
Marini, M. M. (1984). Age and sequencing norms in the transition to adulthood. Social Forces, 63, 229–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, H. M., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marrow, A. J. (1938). Goal tensions and recall: I. Journal of General Psychology, 19, 3–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W. (1989). Age and sex differences in multiple dimensions of self-concept: Preadolescence to adulthood. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 417–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W., Byrne, B. M., & Shavelson, R. J. (1988). A multifaceted academic self-concept: Its hierarchical structure and its relation to academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 366–380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W., Cairns, L., Relich, J., Barnes, J., & Debus, R. L. (1984). The relationship between dimensions of self-attribution and dimension of self-concept. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 3–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marszal-Wisniewska, M. (2002). Model of volitional and temperamental influences on everyday functioning. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 33, 151–157.Google Scholar
Martin, A. J., Marsh, H. W., & Debus, R. L. (2001). A quadripolar need achievement representation of self-handicapping and defensive pessimism. American Educational Research Journal, 38, 583–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1989). Toward a motivational and structural theory or ruminative thought. In Uleman, J. S. & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 306–326). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In Wyer, R. S. Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 9, pp. 1–47). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Martinez, J. A. (2000). Body-weight regulation: Causes of obesity. Proceedings of the Nutritional Society, 59 (3), 337–345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martire, J. C. (1956). Relationship between the self-concept and differences in the strength and generality of achievement motivation. Journal of Personality, 24, 364–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a psychology of being. New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Mason, A., & Blankenship, V. (1987). Power and affiliation motivation, stress, and abuse in intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 203–210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massimini, F., & Carli, M. (1991). Die systematische Erfassung des Flow-Erlebens im Alltag [The systematic assessment of flow experience in everyday life]. In Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.), Die außergewöhnliche Erfahrung im Alltag: Die Psychologie des Flow-Erlebens (pp. 291–312). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T. (2003). The AI project: Historical and ecological contexts. Animal Cognition, 6 (4), 199–211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, G., & Gilliland, K. (1999). The personality theories of H. J. Eysenck and J. A. Gray: A comparative review. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 583–626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1993). The intelligence of emotional intelligence. Intelligence, 17, 433–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, K. U. (1986). Structural constraints on the life course. Human Development, 29, 163–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, K. U., & Carroll, G. (1987). Jobs and classes: Structural constraints on career mobility. European Sociological Review, 3, 14–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, K. U., & Müller, W. (1986). The state and the structure of the life course. Human development and the life course: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 217–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1974). Behavior programs and evolutionary strategies. American Scientist, 62, 650–659.Google ScholarPubMed
Mazur, A. (1985). A biosocial model of status in face-to-face primate groups. Social Forces, 64, 377–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 353–397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAdams, D. P. (1979). Validation of a thematic coding system for the intimacy motive. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1980). A thematic coding system for the intimacy motive. Journal of Research in Personality, 14, 413–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1982a). Intimacy motivation. In Stewart, A. J. (Ed.), Motivation and society (pp. 133–171). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1982b). Experiences of intimacy and power: Relationship between social motives and autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 292–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P., & Constantian, C. A. (1983). Intimacy and affiliation motives in daily living: An experience sampling analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 851–861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P., Healy, S., & Krause, S. (1984). Social motives and patterns of friendship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 828–838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P., & McClelland., D. C. (1983). Social motives and memory. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University, Department of Psychology, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P., & Powers, J. (1981). Themes of intimacy in behavior and thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 573–587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P., & Vaillant, G. E. (1982). Intimacy motivation and psychosocial adjustment: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 46, 586–593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArthur, L. A. (1972). The how and what of why: Some determinants and consequences of causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 171–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McArthur, L. A. (1976). The lesser influence of consensus than distinctiveness information on causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 733–742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1951). Personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1953). The achievement motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts (Irvington/Wiley).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1958a). Methods of measuring human motivation. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 7–42). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1958b). Risk taking in children with high and low need for achievement. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 306–321). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1961). The achieving society. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1965). N achievement and entrepreneurship: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 389–392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C. (1970). The two faces of power. Journal of International Affairs, 24, 29–47.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1971). Assessing human motivation. New York: General Learning.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1975). Power: The inner experience. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1976). New introduction. In McClelland, D. C. (Ed.), The achieving society. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1978). Managing motivation to expand human freedom. American Psychologist, 33, 201–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1979). Inhibited power motivation and high blood pressure in men. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 182–190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C. (1980). Motive dispositions: The merits of operant and respondent measures. In Wheeler, L. (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 10–41). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1982). The need for power, sympathetic activation, and illness. Motivation and Emotion, 6, 31–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1984a). Motives as sources of long-term trends in life and health. In McClelland, D. C. (Ed.), Motives, personality, and society. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1984b). The empire-building motivational syndrome. In McClelland, D. C. (Ed.), Motives, personality, and society: Selected papers (pp. 147–174). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1985a). How motives, skills, and values determine what people do. American Psychologist, 41, 812–825.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1985b). Human motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1987a). Human motivation. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1987b). Biological aspects of human motivation. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 11–19). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1989). Motivational factors in health and disease. American Psychologist, 44, 675–683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C. (1995). Achievement motivation in relation to achievement-related recall, performance, and urine flow, a marker associated with release of vasopressin. Motivation and Emotion, 19, 59–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1999). Human motivation (6th ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Alexander, C., & Marks, E. (1982). The need for power, stress, immune function, and illness among male prisoners. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 61–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achievement motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Boyatzis, R. E. (1982). The leadership motive pattern and long term success in management. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 737–743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Clark, R. A., Roby, T. B., & Atkinson, J. W. (1949). The projective expression of need for achievement on thematic apperception. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 242–255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C., Davis, W. N., Kalin, R., & Wanner, E. (1972). The drinking man.New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Davidson, R. J., Floor, E., & Saron, C. (1980). Stressed power motivation, sympathetic activation, immune function, and illness. Journal of Human Stress, 6, 11–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C., Davidson, R. J., Saron, C., & Floor, E. (1980). The need for power, brain norepinephrine turnover, and learning. Biological Psychology, 10, 93–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Franz, C. E. (1992). Motivational and other sources of work accomplishment in mid-life: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality, 60, 680–707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Jemmott, J. B. (1980). Power motivation, stress, and physical illness. Journal of Human Stress, 6, 6–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ?Psychological Review, 96, 690–702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Liberman, A. M. (1949). The effects of need for achievement on recognition of need related words. Journal of Personality, 18, 236–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Locke, S. E., Jemmot, J. B., Kraus, L., Williams, R. M., & Valeri, C. R. (1985). Motivational syndromes associated with natural killer cell activity and illness. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Maddocks, J. A., & McAdams, D. P. (1985). The need for power, brain noradrenaline turnover, and memory. Motivation and Emotion, 9, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Miron, D. (1979). The impact of achievement motivation training in small businesses. California Management Review, 21 (4).Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Patel, V., Stier, D., & Brown, D. (1987). The relationship of affiliative arousal to dopamine release. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 51–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Pilon, D. A. (1983). Sources of adult motives in patterns of parent behavior in early childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 564–574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Ross, G., & Patel, V. (1985). The effect of an academic examination on salivary norepinephrine and immunoglobuline levels. Journal of Human Stress, 11, 52–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Teague, G. (1975). Predicting risk preference among power related tasks. Journal of Personality, 43, 266–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Watson, R. I. (1973). Power motivation and risk-taking behavior. Journal of Personality, 41, 121–139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C., & Winter, D. G. (1969). Motivating economic achievement. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1987). Validation of the five factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 81–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997). Conceptions and correlates of openness to experience. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 826–848). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Hrebickova, M., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., Avia, M. D., Sanz, J., Sanchez-Bernardos, M. L., Kusdil, M. E., Woodfield, R., Saunders, P. R., & Smith, P. B. (2000). Nature over nurture: Temperament, personality and life span development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78 (1), 173–186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDougall, W. (1908). An introduction to social psychology. London: Methuen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDougall, W. (1932). The energies of men. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
McGuire, W. J. (1966). The current status of cognitive consistency theories. In Feldman, S. (Ed.), Cognitive consistency (pp. 1–46). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
McKeachie, W. J. (1961). Motivation, teaching methods, and college learning. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1961 (pp. 111–142). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
McReynolds, P. (Ed.). (1971). Advances on psychological assessment (Vol. 2). Palo Alto, CA: Science and Behavior Books.Google Scholar
Meaney, M., Aitken, D., Berkel, C., Bhatnagar, S., & Sapolsky, R. (1988). Effect of neonatal handling on age-related impairments associated with the hippocampus. Science, 239, 766–768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehrabian, A. (1969). Measures of achieving tendency. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 29, 445–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehrabian, A. (1970). The development and validation of measures of affiliative tendency and sensitivity to rejection. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 30, 417–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehrabian, A. (1972). Nonverbal communication. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.Google Scholar
Mehrabian, A., & Ksionzky, S. (1974). A theory of affiliation. Lexington, MA: Heath.Google Scholar
Meins, E. (1999). Sensitivity, security, and internal working models: Bridging the transmission gap. Attachment and Human Development, 1, 325–342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior, 17, 83–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mertin, M. (1994). Aufmerksamkeitszuwendung bei vorgenommenen Gelegenheiten [Focusing attention as intended]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich, Germany.Google Scholar
Metalsky, G. I., Halberstadt, L. J., & Abramson, L. Y. (1987). Vulnerability to depressive mood reactions: Toward a more powerful test of the diatheses – stress and causal mediation components of the reformulated theory of depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 386–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metalsky, G. I., Joiner, T. E., Hardin, T. S., & Abramson, L. Y. (1993). Depressive reactions to failure in a naturalistic setting: A test of the hopelessness and self-esteem theories of depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 101–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metcalfe, J., & Jacobs, W. J. (1998). Emotional memory: The effects of stress on “cool” and “hot” memory systems. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 38, 187– 222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meumann, E. (1908/1913). Intelligenz und Wille [Intelligence and volition]. Leipzig, Germany: Quelle & Meyer.Google Scholar
Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L. D., Kay, G. G., Moreland, K. L., Dies, R. R., Eisman, E. J., Kubiszyn, T. W., & Reed, G. M. (2001). Psychological testing and psychological assessment: A review of evidence and issues. American Psychologist, 56, 128–165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, W.-U. (1972). Überlegungen zur Konstruktion eines Fragebogens zur Erfassung von Selbstkonzepten der Begabung [Thoughts on the construction of a questionnaire to assess self-concepts of ability]. Unpublished manuscript, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1973a). Anstrengungsintention in Abhängigkeit von Begabungseinschätzung und Aufgabenschwierigkeit [Intended effort as a function of evaluations of ability and task difficulty]. Archiv für Psychologie, 125, 245–262.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1973b). Leistungsmotiv und Ursachenerklärung von Erfolg und Mißerfolg [The achievement motive and causal attribution of success and failure]. Stuttgart, Germany: Klett.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1976). Leistungsorientiertes Verhalten als Funktion von wahrgenommener eigener Begabung und wahrgenommener Aufgabenschwierigkeit [Achievement-oriented behavior as a function of self-concept of ability and perceived task difficulty]. In Schmalt, H.-D. & Meyer, W.-U. (Eds.), Leistungsmotivation und Verhalten (pp. 101–135). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1978). Der Einfluss von Sanktionen auf Begabungsperzeptionen [The influence of sanctions on perceptions of own ability] (pp. 71–87). Bielefelder Symposium über Attribution. Bielefeld, Germany.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1984a). Das Konzept von der eigenen Begabung [Self-concept of ability]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1984b). Das Konzept von der eigenen Begabung: Auswirkungen, Stabilität und vorauslaufende Bedingungen [Self-concept of ability: Effects, stability, and antecedent conditions]. Psychologische Rundschau, 35, 136–150.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1987). Perceived ability and achievement-related behavior. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 73–86). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1988). Die Rolle von Überraschung im Attributionsprozeß [The role of surprise in the attribution process]. Psychologische Rundschau, 39, 136–147.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Heckhausen, H., & Kemmler, L. (1965). Validierungskorrelate der inhaltsanalytisch erfaßten Leistungsmotivation guter und schwacher Schüler des dritten Schuljahres [Validation correlates of the achievement motivation of good and poor third grade students as measured by picture content analysis]. Psychologische Forschung, 28, 301–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Mittag, W., & Engler, U. (1986). Some effects of praise and blame on perceived ability and affect. Social Cognition, 4, 293–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Niepel, M., Rudolph, U., & Schützwohl, A. (1991). An experimental analysis of surprise. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 295–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Niepel, M., & Schützwohl, A. (1994). Überraschung und Attribution [Surprise and attribution]. In Försterling, F. & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (Eds.), Attribution: Grundlagen und Anwendungen (pp. 105–122). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Reisenzein, R., & Dickhäuser, O. (2004). Inferring ability from blame: Effects of effort- versus liking-oriented cognitive schemata. Psychology Science, 46, 281–293.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Schützwohl, A., & Reisenzein, R. (1993). Einführung in die Emotionspsychologie: Bd. 1. Die Emotionstheorien von Watson, James und Schachter [Introduction to the psychology of emotion: Vol. 1. The emotion theories of Watson, James und Schachter]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Schützwohl, A., & Reisenzein, R. (1999). Einführung in die Emotionspsychologie. Bd. II: Evolutionspsychologische Emotionstheorien [Introduction to the psychology of emotion: Vol. 2. Evolutionary psychology theories of emotion]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., & Starke, E. (1982). Seeking information about one's own ability in relation to self-concept of ability: A field study. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 501–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michalski, N. (2004). Die Wirkung von Vorsätzen in kritischen Situationen im Tenniswettkampf [The effects of implementation intentions in critical situations in tennis competitions]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Konstanz, Germany.Google Scholar
Michotte, A. E. (1912). Note à propos de contributions recentes à la psychologie de la volonté. Études de Psychologie, 1, 193–233.Google Scholar
Michotte, A. E. (1954). Autobiographie. Extrait de “Psychologica Belgia.”Louvain, Belgium: Editions Nauwelaerts.Google Scholar
Michotte, A. E., & Prüm, E. (1910). Étude éxperimentale sur le choix volontaire et ses antecédents immediats. Archives de Psychologie, 10, 119–299.Google Scholar
Midlarsky, E. (1968). Aiding responses: An analysis and review. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 14, 229–260.Google Scholar
Mierke, K. (1955). Wille und Leistung [Volition and achievement]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Milana, S. A. (1981). The effects of naturally occurring depression and induced mood states on social skill. Dissertation Abstracts International, 42, 2541.Google Scholar
Milburn, M. A. (1978). Sources of bias in the prediction of future events. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 21, 17–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. T. (1985). A developmental study of the cognitive basis of performance impairment after failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 529–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. T. (1987). Changes in self academic self concept in early school years: The role of conceptions of ability. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 2, 551–558.Google Scholar
Miller, A. T., & Hom, H. (1990). Influence of extrinsic and ego incentive value on persistence after failure and continuing motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 539–545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. T., & Hom, H. (1997). Conceptions of ability and the interpretation of praise, blame and material rewards. Journal of Experimental Education, 65, 163–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, D. T. (1976). Ego involvement and attribution for success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 901–906.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, D. T., & Ross, M. (1975). Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: Fact or fiction?Psychological Bulletin, 82, 213–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, K. S., & Worchel, P. (1956). The effects of need-achievement and self-ideal discrepancy on performance under stress. Journal of Personality, 25, 176–190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, N. E. (1941). An experimental investigation of acquired drives. Psychological Bulletin, 38, 534–535.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1944). Experimental studies of conflict. In Hunt, J. M. (Ed.), Personality and the behavioral disorders (Vol. I, pp. 431–465). New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1948). Studies of fear as an acquirable drive: I. Fear as motivation and fear-reduction as reinforcement in the learning of new responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 89–101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, N. E. (1951). Learnable drives and rewards. In Stevens, S. S. (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology (pp. 435–472). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1956). Effects of drugs on motivation: The value of using a variety of measures. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 65, 318–333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, N. E. (1959). Liberalization of basic S-R concepts: Extensions to conflict behavior, motivation, and social learning. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. II, pp. 196–292). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1961). Analytical studies of drive and reward. American Psychologist, 16, 739–754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1963). Some reflections on the law of effect produce a new alternative to drive reduction. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1963 (pp. 65–112). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. (1941). Social learning and imitation. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Milne, S., Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2002). Combining motivational and volitional interventions to promote exercise participation: Protection motivation theory and implementation intentions. British Journal of Health Psychology, 7, 163–184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, P. (1970). Physiological psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Mischel, T. (1970). Wundt and the conceptual foundations of psychology. Philosophical and Phenomenological Research, 31, 1–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W., Cantor, N., & Feldman, S. (1996). Principles of self-regulation: The nature of willpower and self-control. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 329–360). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., & Gilligan, C. (1964). Delay of gratification, motivation for the prohibited gratification, and responses to temptation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 411–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246–268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1998). Reconciling processing dynamics and personality dispositions. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 229–258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Smith, R. E. (2004). Introduction to personality (7th ed.). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Mitchell, T. R. (1974). Expectancy models of job satisfaction, occupational preference and effort: A theoretical, methodological, and empirical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 1053–1077.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, T. R. (1982). Expectancy-value models in organizational psychology. In Feather, N. T. (Ed.), Expectations and actions: Expectancy-value models in psychology (pp. 293–312). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mitchell, T. R., & Albright, D. (1972). Expectancy theory predictions of job satisfaction, job effort, job performance, and retention of naval aviation officers. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 8, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, T. R., & Biglan, A. (1971). Instrumentality theories: Current uses in psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 432–454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mittag, H.-D. (1955). Über personale Bedingungen des Gedächtnisses für Handlungen [On the personal conditions of memory for actions]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 158, 40–120.Google Scholar
Modell, J. (1980). Normative aspects of American marriage timing since World War II. Journal of Family History, 5, 210–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Modell, J., Fürstenberg, F. F. J., & Hershberg, T. (1976). Social change and transitions to adulthood in historical perspective. Journal of Family History, 1, 7–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Modell, J., Fürstenberg, F. F. J., & Strong, D. (1978). The timing of marriage in the transition to adulthood: Continuity and change. American Journal of Sociology, 84, 120–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mogenson, G. J., Jones, D. L., & Yim, C. Y. (1980). From motivation to action: Functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. Progress in Neurobiology, 14, 69–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2000). Meaning and motivation. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 131–159). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Moller, A. C., & Elliot, A. J. (2006). The 2 × 2 achievement goal framework: An overview of empirical research. In Mitel, A. V. (Ed.), Focus on educational psychology research (pp. 307–326). New York: Nova Science.Google Scholar
Moneta, G. B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). The effect of perceived challenges and skills on the quality of subjective experience. Journal of Personality, 64, 274–310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monson, T. C., & Snyder, M. (1977). Actors, observers, and the attribution process. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 89–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, C. D., & Murray, H. A. (1935). A method for investigating fantasies: The Thematic Apperceptive Test. Archives of Neurological Psychiatry, 34, 289–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, C. T. (1943). Physiological psychology.New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Morris, J. S., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393(6684), 467–470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moruzzi, G., & Magoun, H. W. (1949). Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. EEG and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1, 455–473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mosher, D. (1968). Measurement of guilt in females by self-report inventories. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 32, 690–695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moulton, R. W. (1958). Notes for a projective measure for fear of failure. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 563–571). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Moulton, R. W. (1965). Effects of success and failure on level of aspiration as related to achievement motives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 399–406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mowrer, H. O. (1939). A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent. Psychological Review, 46, 553–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mowrer, H. O. (1947). On the dual nature of learning: A reinterpretation of “conditioning” and “problem-solving.”Harvard Educational Review, 17, 102–148.Google Scholar
Mowrer, H. O. (1960). Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, G. E., & Pilzecker, A. (1900). Experimentelle Beiträge zur Lehre vom Gedächtnis [Experimental contributions to memory research]. Leipzig, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
Münsterberg, H. (1888). Die Willenshand lung: Ein Beitrag der physiologischen Psychologie [Volitional acts: A contribution to physiological psychology]. Freiburg, Germany: Moler.Google Scholar
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle?Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774–789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, E. J., & Berkun, M. M. (1955). Displacement as a function of conflict. Social Psychology, 51, 47–56.Google ScholarPubMed
Murray, H. A. (1933). The effect of fear upon estimates of the maliciousness of other personalities. Journal of Social Psychology, 4, 310–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality.New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic Apperceptive Test Manual.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Murstein, B. I., & Pryer, R. S. (1959). The concept of projection: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 353–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, J. (1991). Optimales Erleben und die Nutzung der Begabung [Optimal experience and making use of talent]. In Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.), Die außergewöhnliche Erfahrung im Alltag: Die Psychologie des Flow-Erlebens (pp. 326–334). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Neiss, R. (1988). Reconceptualizing arousal: Psychobiological states in motor performance. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 345–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, E. E., & Panksepp, J. (1998). Brain substrates of infant-mother attachment: Contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 22(3), 437–452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, R. J. (2005). An introduction to behavioral endocrinology (3rd ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Nesse, R. M. (2000). Is depression an adaptation?Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 14–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesse, R. M. (2001). Evolution and the capacity for commitment. Volume III in the Russell Sage Foundation Series on Trust. New York: Sage.Google Scholar
Neugarten, B. L. (1979). Time, age, and the life cycle. American Journal of Psychiatry, 136, 887–894.Google ScholarPubMed
Neugarten, B. L., Moore, J. W., & Lowe, J. C. (1965). Age norms, age constraints and adult socialization. American Journal of Sociology, 70, 710–717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Newell, A., & Rosenbloom, P. S. (1981). Mechanisms of skill acquisition and the law of practice. In Anderson, J. R. (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 1–55). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Newtson, D. (1974). Dispositional inference from effects of actions: Effects chosen and effects foregone. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 489–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1975). Causal attributions and other achievement-related cognitions: Effects of task outcome, attainment value, and sex. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 379–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1976). Effort is virtuous, but it's better to have ability: Evaluative responses to perceptions of effort and ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 306–315.Google Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1978). The development of the concepts of effort and ability, perception of own attainment, and the understanding that difficult tasks require more than ability. Child Development, 49, 800–814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1984a). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1984b). Conceptions of ability and achievement motivation. In Ames, R. & Ames, C. (Eds.), Student motivation (pp. 39–73). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1985). Development and its discontents: The differentiation of the concept of ability. In Nicholls, J. G. (Ed.), The development of achievement motivation (pp. 185–218). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nicholls, J. G., & Miller, A. T. (1983). The differentiation of the concepts of difficulty and ability. Child Development, 54, 951–959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niederberger, U., Engemann, A., & Radtke, M. (1987). Umfang der Informationsverarbeitung bei Entscheidungen: Der Einfluss von Gedächtnisbelastung und Handlungsorientierung [Scope of information processing in decision making: The impact of load and action orientation]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 34, 80–100.Google Scholar
Nietzsche, F. (1878). Menschliches, allzumenschliches [Human, all-too-human] (H. Zimmern, Trans.). Dover Publication.Google Scholar
Niitamo, P. (1999). “Surface” and “depth” in human personality: Relations between explicit and implicit motives. Helsinki, Finland: Finish Institute of Occupational Health.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Borgida, E. (1975). Attribution and the psychology of prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 932–943.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Borgida, E., Crandall, R., & Reed, H. (1976). Popular induction: Information is not necessarily informative. In Carroll, J. & Payne, J. (Eds.), Cognitive and social behavior (pp. 113–133). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolen-Hoecksema, S., Girgus, J. S., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1992). Predictors and consequences of childhood symptoms: A 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 405–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 92–104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, D. A. (1980). Twelve issues for cognitive science. Cognitive Science, 4, 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In Davidson, R. J., Schwartz, G. E., & Shapiro, D. (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research (Vol. 4, pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novak, T. P., & Hoffman, D. L. (1997, July). Measuring the flow experience among web users. Paper presented at the Interval Research Corporation, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee.Google Scholar
Nurmi, J. E. (1992). Age differences in adult life goals, concerns and their temporal extension: A life course approach to future-oriented motivation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 487–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nurmi, J. E., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2002). Goal construction, reconstruction and depressive symptoms in a life-span context: The transition from school to work. Journal of Personality, 70, 385–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nurmi, J. E., Salmela-Aro, K., & Koivisto, P. (2002). Goal importance and related achievement beliefs and emotions during the transition from vocational school to work: Antecedents and consequences. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60, 241–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nygard, R. (1975). A reconsideration of the achievement motivation theory. European Journal of Social Psychology, 5, 61–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nygard, R. (1977). Personality, situation, and persistence.Oslo, Norway: Universitetsforlaget.Google Scholar
Nygard, R. (1982). Achievement motives and individual differences in situational specificity of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 319–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1215–1229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, P., Atkinson, J. W., & Horner, M. S. (1966). Motivational implications of ability grouping in schools. In Atkinson, J. W. & Feather, N. T. (Eds.), A theory of achievement motivation (pp. 231–248). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
O'Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., & Andrews, C. (2001). Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 4(1), 95–102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oettingen, G. (1996). Positive fantasy and motivation. In Gollwitzer, P. M. & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), Psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 236–259). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Oettingen, G. (1997). Psychologie des Zukunftsdenkens [The psychology of thinking about the future]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Oettingen, G. (2000). Expectancy effects on behavior depend on self-regulatory thought. Social Cognition, 18, 101–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2000). Das Setzen und Verwirklichen von Zielen [Goal setting and goal striving]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 208(3–4), 406–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Goal setting and goal striving. In Tesser, A. & Schwarz, N. (Eds.), The Blackwell handbook of social psychology (pp. 329–347). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Oettingen, G., Pak, H. J., & Schnetter, K. (2001). Self-regulation of goal-setting: Turning free fantasies about the future into binding goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 736–753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Öhman, A., Fredrikson, M., Hughdal, K., & Rimmö, P. A. (1976). The premise of equipotentiality in human classical conditioning: Conditioned electrodermal responses to potentially phobic stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105, 313–337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olds, J. (1955). Physiological mechanisms of reward. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1955 (pp. 73–139). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Olds, J. (1958). Satiation effects in self-stimulation of the brain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 51, 675–679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olds, J. (1969). The central nervous system and the reinforcement of behavior. American Psychologist, 24, 114–132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olds, J., & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 47, 419–427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olds, J., & Olds, M. (1965). Drives, rewards, and the brain. In Newcomb, T. N. (Ed.), New directions in psychology (Vol. II, pp. 327–404). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Olweus, D. (1976). Der “moderne” Interaktionismus von Person und Situation und seine varianzanalytische Sackgasse [The “modern” interactionism of person and situation and the impasse of variance analysis]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 8, 171–185.Google Scholar
Orbell, S. (2003). Personality systems interaction theory and the theory of planned behavior: Evidence that self-regulatory volitional components enhance enactment of studying behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 95–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orbell, S., Hodgkins, S., & Sheeran, P. (1997). Implementation intentions and the theory of planned behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 945–954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2000). Motivational and volitional processes in action initiation: A field study of the role of implementation intentions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 780–797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orpen, C. (1983). Risk-taking attitudes among Indian, United States, and Japanese managers. Journal of Social Psychology, 120, 283–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orvis, B. R., Cunningham, J. D., & Kelley, H. H. (1975). A closer examination of causal inference: The role of consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 605–616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oster, G., & Alberch, P. (1982). Evolution and bifurcation of developmental programs. Evolution, 36, 444–459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ovsiankina, M. (1928). Die Wiederaufnahme unterbrochener Handlungen [The resumption of interrupted activities]. Psychologische Forschung, 11, 302–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyegbile, T. O. and Marler, C. A. (2005). Winning fights elevates testosterone levels in California mice and enhances future ability to win fights. Hormones and Behavior 48, 259–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Packard, M. G., Cornell, A. H., & Alexander, G. M. (1997). Rewarding affective properties of intra-nucleus accumbens injections of testosterone. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 219–224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational Research, 66, 543–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palfai, T. P. (2002). Action-state orientation and the self-regulation of eating behavior. Eating Behaviors, 3, 249–259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palfai, T. P., McNally, A. M., & Roy, M. (2002). Volition and alcohol-risk reduction: The role of action orientation in the reduction of alcohol-related harm among college student drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 27, 309–317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pang, J. S., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2005). Assessing implicit motives in U.S. college students: Effects of picture type and position, gender and ethnicity, and cross-cultural comparisons. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85(3), 280–294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2003). Feeling the pain of social rejection. Science, 302, 237–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papoušek, H. (1967). Experimental studies of appetitional behavior in human newborns and infants. In Stevenson, H. W., Hess, E. H., & Rheingold, H. L. (Eds.), Early behavior: Comparative developmental approaches (pp. 249–277). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Papoušek, H., & Papoušek, M. (1975). Cognitive aspects of preverbal social interaction between human infants and adults. In Hofer, M. (Ed.), Parent-infant interaction (pp. 241–269). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google ScholarPubMed
Papoušek, H., & Papoušek, M. (1987). Intuitive parenting: A dialectic counterpart to the infant's integrative competence. In Osofsky, J. D. (Eds.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 669–720). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Parsons, J. E., & Ruble, D. (1977). The development of achievement-related expectancies. Child Development, 48, 1075–1079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Passman, R. H., & Erck, T. (1978). Permitting maternal contact through vision alone: Films of mothers for promoting play and locomotion. Developmental Psychology, 14(5), 512–516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patten, R. L., & White, L. A. (1977). Independent effects of achievement motivation and overt attribution on achievement behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 39–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, R. (2001). The school-to-work transition: A cross-national perspective. Journal of Economic Literature, 39, 34–92.Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes.London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Peak, H. (1955). Attitude and motivation. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1955 (pp. 149–189). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Pecina, S., Cagniard, B., Berridge, K. C., Aldridge, J. W., & Zhuang, X. (2003). Hyperdopaminergic mutant mice have higher “wanting” but not “liking” for sweet rewards. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(28), 9395–9402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peck, J. W., & Blass, E. M. (1975). Localization of thirst and antidiuretic osmoreceptors by intracranial injections in rats. American Journal of Physiology, 5, 1501–1509.Google Scholar
Pekrun, R. (1993). Entwicklung von schulischer Aufgabenmotivation in der Sekundarstufe: Ein erwartungswert-theoretischer Ansatz [Development of task-related motivation in secondary school: An expectancy-value approach]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 7, 87–98.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. (1993). Putting stress into words: Health, linguistic, and therapeutic implications. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 539–548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penner, D. D., Fitch, G., & Weick, K. E. (1966). Dissonance and the revision of choice criteria. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 701–705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perin, C. I. (1942). Behavioral potentiality as a joint function of the amount of training and the degree of hunger at the time of extinction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30, 93–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perloff, L. S., & Fetzer, B. K. (1986). Self-other judgements and perceived vulnerability of victimization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 502–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérusse, D. (1993). Cultural and reproductive success in industrial societies: Testing the relationship at the proximate and ultimate levels. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 267–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérusse, D. (1994). Mate choice in modern societies: Testing evolutionary hypotheses with behavioural data. Human Nature, 5, 255–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pervin, L. A. (Ed.). (1989). Goal concepts in personality and social psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Peterson, B. E., & Stewart, A. J. (1993). Generativity and social motives in young adults. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 186–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psychological Review, 91, 347–374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, C., Semmel, A., Baeyer, C., Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1982). The attributional style questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6, 287–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfaffmann, C. (1982). Taste: A model of incentive motivation. In Pfaff, D. W. (Ed.), The physiological mechanisms of motivation (pp. 61–97). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfänder, A. (1911). Motive und Motivation [Motives and motivation]. In Pfänder, A. (Ed.), Münchener Philosophische Abhandlungen (Festschrift für Theodor Lipps) (pp. 163–195). Leipzig, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
Pfaus, J. G., Damsma, G., Wenkstern, D., & Fibiger, H. C. (1995). Sexual activity increases dopamine transmission in the nucleus accumbens and striatum of female rats. Brain Research, 693(1–2), 21–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfister, R. (2002). Flow im Alltag [Flow in everyday life]. Berne, Switzerland: Lang.Google Scholar
Phares, E. J. (1976). Locus of control in personality. Morristown, NJ: General Learning.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1936). Le naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant. Neuchâtel, Switzerland: Delachaux et Nestlé.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pikowsky, B. (1988). Lob im Unterricht: Lehrer-und Schülerkognitionen im Vergleich [Praise in school classes: Comparison of teacher and student cognitions]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 2, 251–257.Google Scholar
Pintrich, P. R., & Groot, E. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pintrich, P. R., & Garcia, T. (1991). Student goal orientation and self-regulation in the college classroom. In Maehr, M. & Pintrich, P. R. (Eds.), Advance in motivation and achievement: Goals and self-regulatory processes (Vol. 7, pp. 371–402). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Pizzagalli, D. A., Regard, M., & Lehmann, D. (1999). Rapid emotional face processing in the human right and left brain hemispheres: An ERP study. NeuroReport, 10, 2691–2698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plath, D. W., & Ikeda, K. (1975). After coming of age: Adult awareness of age norms. In Williams, T. R. (Ed.), Socialization and communication in primacy groups (pp. 107–123). The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R. (2004). Genetics and developmental psychology. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 341–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Craig, I. W., & McGuffin, P. (Eds.). (2003). Behavioral genetics in the postgenomic era. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., & Roew, D. C. (1979). Genetic and environmental psychology. Developmental Psychology, 15, 62–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plutchi, R. (1980). Emotion: A psychoevolutionary synthesis.New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Pomerantz, E. M., & Ruble, D. (1997). Distinguishing multiple dimensions of conceptions of ability: Implications for self-evaluations. Child Development, 68, 1165–1180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poppe, P. (2002). Nutzung von Kovariationsinformationen im Attributionsprozess: Ein Vergleich depressiver und nichtdepressiver Erwachsener [Use of covariation information in the attribution process: A comparison of depressive and nondepressive adults]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Gießen, Germany.Google Scholar
Poppe, P., Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Pelster, A. (2005). Attributionsstilfragebogen für Erwachsene (ASF-E) [Attribution style questionnaire for adults (ASF-E)]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Porter, L. W., & Lawler, E. E. (1968). Managerial attitudes and performance. Homewood, IL: Irwin-Dorsey.Google Scholar
Porter, R. H. (1998). Olfaction and human kin recognition. Genetica, 104(3), 259–263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pösl, I. (1994). Wiederaufnahme unterbrochener Handlungen: Effekte der Bewusstseinslagen des Abwägens und Planens [Mindset effects on the resumption of disrupted activities]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Munich, Germany.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1992). Attentional mechanisms and conscious experience. In Milner, A. D. & Rugg, M. D. (Eds.), The neuropsychology of consciousness (pp. 91–111). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Postman, L., & Solomon, R. L. (1949). Perceptual sensitivity to completed and incompleted tasks. Journal of Personality, 18, 347–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulin, M., Haase, C. M., & Heckhausen, J. (2005). Engagement and disengagement across the life-span: An analysis of two process models of development regulation. In Greve, W., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (Eds.), The adaptive self: Personal continuity and intentional self-development (pp. 117–135). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber.Google Scholar
Pribram, K. H. (1976). Self-consciousness and intentionality. In Schwartz, G. E. & Shapiro, D. (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research (Vol. 1, pp. 51–100). New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pritchard, R. D., & Sanders, M. S. (1973). The influence of valence, instrumentality, and expectancy of effort and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 57, 55–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruitt, D. J., & Insko, C. A. (1980). Extension of the Kelley attribution model: The role of comparison-object consensus, target-object consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 39–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puca, R. M. (2001). Preferred difficulty and subjective probability in different action phases. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 307–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puca, R. M., & Schmalt, H. (2001). The influence of the achievement motive on spontaneous thoughts in pre- and postdecisional action phases. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 302–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pusey, A., Williams, J., & Goodall, J. (1997). The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science, 277, 828–831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pyszczynski, T. A., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Toward an integration of cognitive and motivational perspectives on social inference: A biased hypotheses-testing model. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 20). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Radloff, L. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramey, C., & Finkelstein, N. (1978). Contingent stimulation and infant competence. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 3, 89–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rank, O. (1945). Will therapy and truth and reality. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Rapaport, D. (1959). The structure of psychoanalytic theory: A systematizing attempt. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. III, pp. 55–183). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Rapaport, D. (1960). On the psychoanalytic theory of motivation. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1960 (pp. 173–247). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Raps, C. S., Peterson, C., Reinhard, K. E., Abramson, L. Y., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1982). Attributional style among depressed patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 102–108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasch, G. (1960). Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. Copenhagen, Denmark: Nielson & Lydicke.Google Scholar
Raup, R. B. (1925). Complacency, the foundation of human behavior.New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Raven, B. H. (1974). The comparative analysis of power and power preference. In Tedeschi, J. T. (Ed.), Perspectives on social power. Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
Raynor, J. O. (1969). Future orientation and motivation of immediate activity: An elaboration of the theory of achievement motivation. Psychological Review, 76, 606–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raynor, J. O. (1974). Future orientation in the study of achievement motivation. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 121–154). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Raynor, J. O., & Entin, E. E. (1982). Motivation, career striving and aging. Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Raynor, J. O., & Roeder, G. P. (1987). Motivation and future orientation: Task and time effects for achievement motivation. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 61–71). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reif, M. (1970). Leistungsmotivation in Abhängigkeit vom Erziehungsverhalten der Mutter [Achievement motivation as a function of the mother's parenting]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Reitman, W. R. (1960). Motivational induction and the behavioral correlates of the achievement and affiliation motives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 8–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Remy, K. (2000). Entwicklung eines Fragebogens zum Flow-Erleben [Development of a questionnaire on flow experience]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bielefeld, Germany.Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1968). Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 1–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, R. A. (1972). Informational variables in Pavlovian conditioning. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 1–46). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current theory and research (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
Resnik, S. M.Gottesman, I. I., & McGue, M. (1993). Sensation seeking in opposite-sex twins: An effect of prenatal hormones?Behavior Genetics, 23, 323–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rethorts, S. (1994). Attribution und Emotion in der sportpsychologischen Forschung [Attribution and emotion in sports psychology research]. In Försterling, F. & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (Eds.), Attributionstheorie (pp. 163–183). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Reuman, D. A. (1982). Ipsative behavioral variability and the quality of thematic apperceptive measurement of the achievement motive. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1098–1110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reuman, D. A., Alwin, D. F., & Veroff, J. (1984). Assessing the validity of the achievement motive in the presence of random measurement error. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1347–1386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Revelle, W. (1986). Motivation and efficiency of cognitive performance. In Brown, D. R. & Veroff, J. (Eds.), Frontiers of motivational psychology (pp. 107–127). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revelle, W., & Michaels, E. J. (1976). The theory of achievement motivation revisited: The implications of inertial tendencies. Psychological Review, 83, 394–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, W. F., & Anderson, J. E. (1961). Choice behavior in a T-maze as a function of deprivation period and magnitude or reward. Psychological Reports, 8, 131–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1975). Zeitstabilität und Steuerbarkeit von Ursachen schulischer Leistung in der Sicht des Lehrers [Temporal stability and controllability of the causes of school outcomes from the teacher's perspective]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 7, 180–194.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1980). Leistungsbewertung und Lernmotivation [Achievement evaluation and motivation to learn]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1989). Zweck und Tätigkeit [Purpose and activity]. Göttingen, Ger many: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1991). Flow-experience when motorcycling: A study of a special human condition. In Brendicke, R. (Ed.), Safety, environment, future: Proceedings of the 1991 International Motorcycle Conference (pp. 349–362). Bochum, Germany: IfZ.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1993). Anreize engagiert betriebener Freizeitaktivitäten. Ein Systematisierungsversuch [An attempt to systematize the incentives of leisure time activities]. Unpublished manuscript, University of Potsdam, Germany.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1995). Motivation. Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1996). Flow-Erleben, Freude an riskantem Sport und andere “unvernünftige” Motivationen [Flow experience, enjoyment of dangerous sports and other “unwise” motivations]. In Kuhl, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Motivation, Volition und Handlung. Enzyklopädie der Psychologie C(IV(4 (pp. 101–118). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (2001). Bezugsnormorientierung [Reference norm orientations]. In Rost, D. (Ed.), Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie (pp. 55–62). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (2002a). Freude am Kompetenzerwerb, Flow-Erleben und motivpassende Ziele [Enjoyment of learning, flow experience and motive-congruent goals]. In Salisch, M. V. (Ed.), Emotionale Kompetenz entwickeln (pp. 179–206). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (2002b). Motivationale Kompetenz [Motivational competence]. Paper presented at the 22nd Colloquium on Motivational Psychology (MPK), Siegen, Germany. Retrieved July 12, 2007, from www.psych.uni-potsdam.de/people/rheinberg/personal/lectures-d.html.
Rheinberg, F. (2004a). Motivationsdiagnostik [Measurement of motivation]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (2004b, July). Motivational competence and flow-experience. Paper presented at the 2nd European Conference of Positive Psychology, Verbania, Italy. Retrieved July 12, 2007, from www.psych.uni-potsdam.de/people/rheinberg/personal/lectures-d.html.
Rheinberg, F. (2006). Motivation (6th ed.). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Duscha, R., & Michels, U. (1980). Zielsetzung und Kausalattribution in Abhängigkeit vom Leistungsvergleich [Goal setting and causal attribution as a function of performance comparisons]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 12, 177– 189.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Iser, I., & Pfauser, S. (1997). Freude am Tun und(oder zweckorientiertes Schaffen? Zur transsituativen Konsistenz und konvergenten Validität der Anreiz-Fokus-Skala [Doing something for fun and/or for gain? Transsituational consistency and convergent validity of the Incentive Focus Scale]. Diagnostica, 42, 174–191.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., & Krug, S. (2005). Motivationsförderung im Schulalltag [Fostering motivation in school] (3rd ed.). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Lührmann, J.-V., & Wagner, H. (1977). Bezugsnormorientierung von Schülern der 5. bis 13. Klassenstufe bei der Leistungsbeurteilung [Reference norm orientations in 5th to 13th graders]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogischer Psychologie, 9, 90–93.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., & Manig, Y. (2003). Was macht Spaß am Graffiti-Sprayen? Eine induktive Anreizanalyse [What is enjoyable about graffiti spraying? An inductive incentive analysis]. Report Psychologie, 4, 222–234.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Manig, Y., Kliegl, R., Engeser, S., & Vollmeyer, R. (2007). Flow bei der Arbeit, doch Glück in der Freizeit: Zielausrichtung, Flow und Glücksgefühle [Flow at work, but happiness at leisure: Goal directedness, flow and feelings of happiness]. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 51, 105–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Schmalt, H., & Wasser, I. (1978). Ein Lehrerunterschied, der etwas ausmacht [A difference between teachers that matters]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 10(1), 3–7.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. & Tramp, N. (2006) Anreizanalyse intensiver Nutzung von Computen in der Freizeit [Analysis of the incentive for intensive leisure-time use of computers]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 214, 97–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rheinberg, F., & Vollmeyer, R. (2003). Flow-Erleben in einem Computerspiel unter experimentell variierten Bedingungen [Flow experience in a computer game under experimentally controlled conditions]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 114, 161–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rheinberg, F., & Vollmeyer, R. (2004). Flow-Erleben bei der Arbeit und in der Freizeit [Flow experience at work and in leisure time]. In Wegge, J. & Schmidt, K.-H. (Eds.), Förderung von Arbeitsmotivation und Gesundheit in Organisationen (pp. 163–180). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Burns, B. D. (2000). Motivation and self-regulated learning. In Heckhausen, J. (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 81–108). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Engeser, S. (2003). Die Erfassung des Flow-Erlebens [Measuring flow experience]. In Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. & Rheinberg, F. (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept(Tests and Trends Vol. 2, pp. 261–279). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., & Weich, K. (1988). Wie gefährlich ist Lob? Eine Untersuchung zum “paradoxen Effekt” von Lehrersanktionen [How risky is it to praise students? A study concerning “paradox effects” of teachers' sanctions]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 2, 227–233.Google Scholar
Rheingold, H., & Eckerman, C. (1969). The infant's free entry into a new environment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8(2), 271–283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rholes, W. S., Blackwell, J., Jordan, C., & Walters, C. (1980). A developmental study of learned helplessness. Developmental Psychology, 16, 616–624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rholes, W. S., &. Ruble, D. (1984). Children's understanding of dispositional characteristics of others. Child Development, 55, 550–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richter, C. P. (1927). Animal behavior and internal drives. Quarterly Review of Biology, 2, 307–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1997). Genetic and environmental influences on personality: A study of twins reared together using the self- and peer-report NEO-FFI scales. Journal of Personality, 65, 449–475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (1978). Effects of caregiver behavior on habituation rate and self-efficacy in infants. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1, 105–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rind, B., & Kipnis, D. (1999). Changes in self-perception as a result of successfully persuading others. Journal of Social Issues, 55, 141–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rindfuss, R. R., Swicegood, C. G., & Rosenfeld, R. A. (1987). Disorder in the life course: How common and does it matter?American Sociological Review, 52, 785–801.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rinn, W. E. (1984). The neuropsychology of facial emotions: A review of the neurological and psychological mechanism for producing facial expressions. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 52–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riskind, J. H. (1984). They stoop to conquer: Guiding and self-regulatory functions of physical posture after success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 479–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, B. W., & Caspi, A. (2003). The cumulative continuity model of personality development: Striking a balance between continuity and change in personality traits across the life course. Understanding human development: Dialogues with lifespan psychology (pp. 183–214). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2000). The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: An incentive-sensitization view. Addiction, 95(Suppl 2), S91–S117.Google Scholar
Roe, A., & Siegelman, M. (1964). The origin of interests. APGA Inquiry Studies, 1, 98.Google Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person: A therapist's view of psychotherapy.Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Rogers, E. M., & Svenning, L. (1969). Modernization among peasants: The impact of communication.New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 124, 207–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B., & Wertsch, J.(1984). Children's learning in the “zone of proximal development.” New Directions for Child Development, 23.Google Scholar
Röhrle, B., Hedke, J., & Leibold, S. (1994). Persönliche Projekte zur Herstellung und Pflege sozialer Beziehungen bei depressiven und nicht-depressiven Personen [Personal projects serving the establishment and maintenance of social relations in depressive and nondepressive adults]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, 23, 43–51.Google Scholar
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values.New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and emotion. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 284–294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 11–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, E. T. (2005). Taste, olfactory, and food texture processing in the brain, and the control of food intake. Physiology & Behavior, 85(1), 45–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roney, J. R., Mahler, S. V., & Maestripieri, D. (2003). Behavioral and hormonal responses of men to brief interactions with women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 365–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rook, K., Sorkin, D., & Zettel, A. (2004). Stress in social relationships: Coping and adaptation across the lifespan. In Lang, F. R. & Fingerman, K. L. (Eds.), Growing together: Personal relationships across the lifespan (pp. 210–239). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rosahl, S. K., Tennigkeit, M., Kuhl, J., & Haschke, R. (1993). Handlungskontrolle und langsame Hirnpotentiale: Untersuchungen zum Einfluss subjektiv kritischer Wörter (Erste Ergebnisse) [Action control and slow brain potentials: Studies on the impact of subjectively emotional words (first findings)]. Zeitschrift für Medizinische Psychologie, 2, 1–8.Google Scholar
Rose, M. (1991). Evolutionary biology of aging. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rose, R. M., Holaday, J. W., & Bernstein, I. S. (1971). Plasma testosterone, dominance rank, and aggressive behavior in male rhesus monkeys. Nature, 231, 898–904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, B. C. (1959). Race, ethnicity, and the achievement syndrome. American Sociological Review, 24, 47–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosen, B. C., & Andrade, D' R. (1959). The psychosocial origins of achievement motivation. Sociometry, 22, 185–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenbaum, R. M. (1972). A dimensional analysis of the perceived causes of success and failure. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, G. J. (1956). Cognitive structure and attitudinal affect. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53, 367–372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenzweig, S. (1933). Preferences in the repetition of successful and unsuccessful activities as a function of age and personality. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 42, 423–441.Google Scholar
Rosenzweig, S. (1941). Need-persistive and ego-defensive reactions to frustration as demonstrated by an experiment on repression. Psychological Review, 48, 347–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenzweig, S. (1943). Experimental study of “repression” with special reference to need-persistive and ego-defensive reactions to frustration. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 64–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenzweig, S. (1945). Further comparative data on repetition choice after success and failure as related to frustration tolerance. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 66, 75–81.Google Scholar
Rösler, H.-D. (1955). Über das Behalten von Handlungen schwachsinniger und normaler Kinder [Retention of actions by feeble-minded and normal children]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 158, 161–231.Google Scholar
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his short-comings: Distortions in the attribution process. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173–220). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rotenberg, V. S. (1993). Richness against freedom: Two hemisphere functions and the problem of creativity. European Journal for High Ability, 4, 11–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotenberg, V. S. (2004). The peculiarity of the right hemisphere function in depression: Solving the paradoxes. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 28, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothermund, K., & Brandstädter, J. (2003a). Coping with deficits and losses in later life: From compensatory action to accommodation. Psychology and Aging, 18, 896–905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothermund, K., & Brandstädter, J. (2003b). Depression in later life: Cross sequential patterns and possible determinants. Psychology and Aging, 18, 80–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J. B. (1954). Social learning and clinical psychology.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J. B. (1955). The role of the psychological situation in determining the direction of human behavior. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1955 (pp. 245–269). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Rotter, J. B. (1960). Some implications of a social learning theory for the prediction of goal directed behavior from testing procedures. Psychological Review, 67, 301–316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80(1, Whole no. 609), 1–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J. B. (1982). Social learning theory. In Feather, N. T. (Ed.), Expectations and actions (pp. 241–260). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rotter, J. B., Chance, J. E., & Phares, E. J. (1972). Applications of a social learning theory of personality.New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Rotter, J. B., Liverant, S., & Crowne, D. P. (1961). The growth and extinction of expectancies in chance controlled and skilled tasks. Journal of Psychology, 52, 161–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J. B., Seeman, M. R., & Liverant, S. (1962). Internal versus external control of reinforcements: A major variable in behavior theory. In Washburn, W. F. (Ed.), Decisions, values, and groups (Vol. 2, pp. 473–516). New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Rovee, C. K., & Fagan, J. (1976). Extended conditioning and 24 hour retention in infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P. (1976). The evolution of intelligence and access to the cognitive unconscious. In Sprague, J. M. & Epstein, A. N. (Eds.), Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology (pp. 245–277). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ruble, D. N., Boggiano, A., Feldman, N. S., & Loebl, J. H. (1980). A developmental analysis of the role of social comparison in self-evaluation. Developmental Psychology, 16, 105–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruble, D. N., & Feldman, N. S. (1976). Order of consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency information and causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 930–937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (2002). Conceptualization, measuring and evaluation the developmental course of gender differentiation: Compliments, queries and quandaries. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(269), 148–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruble, D. N., Parsons, J. E., & Ross, M. (1976). Self-evaluative responses of children in an achievement setting. Child Development, 48, 1362–1368.Google Scholar
Rudolph, U., Roesch, S. C., Weiner, B., & Greitmeyer, T. (2004). Responsibility and help-giving: A meta-analytic review. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 815–848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruhland, D., & Feld, S. (1977). The development of achievement motivation in black and white children. Child Development, 48, 1362–1368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, B. (1938). Power.London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 3–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). When rewards compete with nature: The undermining of intrinsic motivation and self-regulation. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 14–54). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M., Kuhl, J., & Deci, E. (1997). Nature and autonomy: An organizational view of social and neurobiological aspects of self-regulation in behavior and development. Development and Psychopathology, 9(4), 701–728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadalla, E. K., Kenrick, D. T., & Vershure, B. (1987). Dominance and heterosexual attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 730–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sader, M., & Keil, W. (1968). Faktorenanalytische Untersuchungen zur Projektion der Leistungsmotivation [Factor analytic studies on the projection of achievement motivation]. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 120, 25–53.Google Scholar
Sader, M., & Specht, H. (1967). Leistung, Motivation und Leistungsmotivation: Korrelationsstatistische Untersuchungen zur Leistungsmotivmessung nach Heckhausen [Achievement, motivation and achievement motivation: Correlational studies on achievement motivation after Heckhausen]. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 119, 90–130.Google Scholar
Salamone, J. D. (1994). The involvement of nucleus accumbens dopamine in appetitive and aversive motivation. Behavioural Brain Research, 61, 117–133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sameroff, A. J. (1983). Developmental systems: Contexts and evolution. Handbook of Child Psychology, 1(4), 237–294.Google Scholar
Sanford, R. N. (1937). The effects of abstinence from food upon imaginal processes: A preliminary experiment. Journal of Psychology, 3, 145–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanford, R. N., & Risser, J. (1948). What are the conditions of self-defensive forgetting?Journal of Personality, 17, 244–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sansone, C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sansone, C., & Smith, J. L. (2000). Interest and self-regulation: The relation between having to and wanting to. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 343–372). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sansone, C. D., Sachau, A., & Weir, C. (1989). Effects of instruction on intrinsic interest: The importance of context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 819–829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapolsky, R. M. (1987). Stress, social status, and reproductive physiology in free-living baboons. In Crews, D. (Ed.), Psychobiology and reproductive behavior: An evolutionary perspective (pp. 291–322). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Sapolsky, R. M. (1992). Stress, the aging brain, and the mechanism of neuron death. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. (1996). The language of personality: Lexical reflections on the five-factor model. In Wiggins, J. S. (Ed.), The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives(pp. 21–50). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sawusch, J. R. (1974). Computer simulation of the influence of ability and motivation on test performance and cumulative achievement and the relation between them. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 425–438). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424–435.Google ScholarPubMed
Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501–518.Google Scholar
Schallberger, U. (2000). Qualität des Erlebens in Arbeit und Freizeit: Eine Zwischenbilanz [Quality of experience in work and leisure: An interim assessment]. Berichte aus der Abteilung Angewandte Psychologie, No. 31. Zürich, Switzerland: University of Zürich.Google Scholar
Schallberger, U., & Pfister, R. (2001). Flow-Erleben in Arbeit und Freizeit: Eine Untersuchung zum Paradox der Arbeit mit der Experience Sampling Method [Flow experiences in work and leisure: An experience sampling study about the paradox of work]. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 45, 176–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheffer, D. (2000). Entwicklungsbedingungen Impliziter Motive: Bindung, Leistung und Macht [Developmental conditions of implicit motives: Affiliation, achievement, and power]. Doctoral dissertation, University of Osnabrück, Germany. Retrieved July 12, 2007, from http://elib.ub.uni-osnabrueck.de/publications/diss/E-Diss150_thesis.pdf.Google Scholar
Scheffer, D. (2003). Die Messung impliziter Motive [The measurement of implicit motives]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Scheffer, D. (2005). Implizite Motive [Implicit motives]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Scheffer, D., & Kuhl, J. (2006). Erfolgreich motivieren [Successful motivation]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Scheffer, D., Kuhl, J., & Eichstaedt, J. (2003). Der Operante Motiv-Test (OMT): Inhaltsklassen, Auswertung, psychometrische Kennwerte und Validierung [The Operant Motive Test (OMT): Categories, interpretation, psychometric properties, and validation]. In Rheinberg, F. & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept (pp. 151–168). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4, 219–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R. (1981). Über die Vernachlässigung der Emotion in der Psychologie [On the neglect of emotion in psychology]. In Michaelis, M. (Ed.), Bericht über den 32. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie, Zürich 1980 (pp. 304–317). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In Scherer, K. R. & Ekman, P. (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293–317). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schiefele, U. (1996). Motivation und Lernen mit Texten [Motivation and learning from texts]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schiefele, U., & Köller, O. (2001). Intrinsische und extrinsische Motivation [Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation]. In Rost, D. H. (Ed.), Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie (pp. 304–310). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Schiefele, U., & Rheinberg, F. (1997). Motivation and knowledge acquisition: Searching for mediating processes. In Maehr, M. L. & Pintrich, P. R. (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10, pp. 251–301). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Schiefele, U., & Urhahne, D. (2000). Motivationale und volitionale Bedingungen der Studienleistung [Motivational and volitional conditions of college outcomes]. In Schiefele, U. & Wild, K.-P. (Eds.), Interesse und Lernmotivation: Untersuchungen zu Entwicklung, Förderung und Wirkung (pp. 183–205). Münster, Germany: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Schlenker, B. R., & Leary, M. R. (1982). Social anxiety and self-presentation: A conceptualization and model. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 641–669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmajuk, N. A., & Buhusi, C. V. (1997). Stimulus configuration, occasion setting, and the hippocampus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 235–257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmalt, H. (1975). Independence training and various aspects of achievement motivation. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 7, 24–37.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1973). Die GITTER-Technik – ein objektives Verfahren zur Messung des Leistungsmotivs bei Kindern [The Grid technique – an objective technique to measure children's achievement motivation]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 5, 231–252.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1976a). Das LM-GITTER. Handanweisung [The AM-Grid. Manual]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1976b). Die Messung des Leistungsmotivs [Measurement of the achievement motive]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1978). Machtmotivation [Power motivation]. Psychologische Rundschau, 30, 269–285.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1979). Leistungsthematische Kognitionen. II: Kausalattribuierungen, Erfolgserwartungen und Affekte [Achievement-related cognitions. II: Causal attributions, expectancies of success, and affect]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 26, 509–531.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1986). Das Machtmotiv und Verantwortlichkeitsattribution für interpersonale Ereignisse [The power motive and attribution of responsibility for interpersonal events]. Psychologische Beiträge, 28, 533–550.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1987). Power motivation and the perception of control. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 101–113). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1996). Zur Kohärenz von Motivation und Kognition [The coherence of motivation and cognition]. In Kuhl, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie. Motivation, Volition und Handeln (pp. 241–273). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1999). Assessing the achievement motive using the Grid technique. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 109–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (2003). Leistungsmotivation im Unterricht: Über den Einsatz des LM-Gitters in der Schule [Achievement motivation in the classroom: Implementing the AM Grid in schools]. In Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. & Rheinberg, F. (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept (pp. 105–127). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H. D., & Langens, T. (1999). Projective, semiprojective and self-report. Measures of human motivation predict private cognitive events: Strivings, memories, and daydreams. Unpublished manuscript, University of Wuppertal, Germany.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D., & Sokolowski, K. (2000). Zum gegenwärtigen Stand der Motivdiagnostik [The current status of motive measurement]. Diagnostica, 46, 115–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D., & Sokolowski, K. (2004). Motivation. In Spada, H. (Ed.), Allgemeine Psychologie (3rd ed.). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D., Sokolowski, K., & Langens, T. (2000). Das Multi-Motiv-Gitter (MMG) [The Multi-Motive Grid (MMG)]. Lisse, Netherlands: Swets.Google Scholar
Schmidt, F. L. (1973). Implications of a measurement problem for expectancy theory research. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 10, 243–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnackers, U., & Kleinbeck, U. (1975). Machtmotiv und machtthematisches Verhalten in einem Verhandlungsspiel [The power motive and power-related behavior in a con game]. Archiv für Psychologie, 127, 300–319.Google Scholar
Schneider, E. L., & Rowe, J. (1990). Handbook of the biology of aging. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schneider, K. (1971). Leistungs- und Risikoverhalten in Abhängigkeit von situativen und überdauernden Komponenten der Leistungsmotivation: Kritische Untersuchungen zu einem Verhaltensmodell [Achievement-related and risk-taking behavior as a function of situational and enduring components of achievement motivation: Critical investigations of a behavioral model]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Schneider, K. (1973). Motivation unter Erfolgsrisiko [Motivation under the risk of success]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schneider, K. (1974). Subjektive Unsicherheit und Aufgabenwahl [Subjective uncertainty and task choice]. Archiv für Psychologie, 126, 147–169.Google Scholar
Schneider, K. (1996). Intrinsisch (autotelisch) motiviertes Verhalten – dargestellt an den Beispielen des Neugierverhaltens sowie verwandter Verhaltenssysteme (Spielen und leistungsmotiviertes Handeln) [Intrinsically (autotelically) motivated behavior – The examples of curiosity and related behavioral systems (playing and achievement-motivated behavior)]. In Kuhl, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Motivation, Volition und Handlung: Enzyklopädie der Psychologie C/IV/4 (pp. 119–153). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., & Dittrich, W. (1990). Evolution und Funktion von Emotionen [Evolution and the function of emotions]. In Scherer, K. R. (Ed.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Psychologie der Emotion (pp. 41–114). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., & Heckhausen, H. (1981). Subjective uncertainty and task preference. In Day, H. I. (Ed.), Advances in intrinsic motivation and aesthetics (pp. 149–167). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, K., & Kreuz, A. (1979). Die Effekte unterschiedlicher Anstrengung auf die Mengen- und Güteleistung bei einer einfachen und schweren Zahlensymbolaufgabe [The effects of effort on the quantity and quality of performance outcomes on easy and difficult number symbol tasks]. Psychologie und Praxis, 23, 34–42.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., & Schmalt, H.-D. (1994). Motivation (2nd ed.). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., & Schmalt, H.-D. (2000). Motivation (3rd ed.). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., & Schmalt, H.-D. (2002). Motivation (4th ed.). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., & Unzner, L. (1992). Preschoolers' attention and emotion in an achievement and an effect game: A longitudindal study. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 37–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, K., Wegge, J., & Konradt, U. (1993). Motivation und Leistung [Motivation and achievement]. In Beckmann, J., Strang, H., & Hahn, E. (Eds.), Aufmerksamkeit und Energetisierung: Facetten von Konzentration und Leistung (pp. 101–131). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schneider, R. J., Hough, L. M., & Dunnette, M. D. (1996). Broadsided by broad traits: How to sink science in five dimensions or less. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 17, 639–655.3.0.CO;2-9>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnierla, J. C. (1959). An evolutionary and developmental theory of biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1959 (pp. 1–42). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Schöne, C., Dickhäuser, O., Spinath, B., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2004). Zielorientierung und Bezugsnormorientierung: zum Zusammenhang zweier Konzepte [Goal orientation and reference-norm orientation: Two related constructs?]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 18, 93–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schore, A. N. (2003). Affect regulation and the repair of self. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). An experimental approach to anxiety, escape, and avoidance behavior. In Hoch, P. M. & Zubin, J. (Eds.), Anxiety (pp. 70–99). New York: Grune & Stratton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schroth, M. L. (1988). Relationships between achievement-related motives, extrinsic conditions, and task performance. Journal of Social Psychology, 127, 39–48.Google Scholar
Schubert, C. (1986). Motivationsanalysen zur Interaktion mit Computern [Motivational analyses of interaction with computers]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Schülein, J. A. (1989). Symbiotische Beziehungen und gesellschaftliche Entwicklung [Symbiotic relations and societal development]. Psyche, 43, 1007–1028.Google Scholar
Schüler, J. (2002). Ein hierarchisches Modell der Anschlussmotivation: Hoffnung-auf-Anschluss und Furcht-vor-Zurückweisung und die Selbstregulation von Zielsetzungen [A hierarchical model of affiliation motivation: Hope for affiliation and fear of rejection and self-regulation of goal setting]. Doctoral dissertation, University of Wuppertal, Germany. Retrieved July 13, 2007, from http://elpub.bib.uni-wuppertal.de/edocs/dokumente/fb03/diss2002/schueler/f030202.pdf.Google Scholar
Schuler, H., & Prochaska, M. (2000). Entwicklung und Konstruktvalidierung eines berufsbezogenen Leistungsmotivationstests [Development and construct validation of a job-related achievement motivation test]. Diagnostica, 46, 61–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C. (in press). Implicit motives. In John, O. P., Robins, R. W. & Pervin, L. A. (Eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. (3 ed.). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C. (2001a). Manual for the assessment of hope of success and fear of failure: English translation of Heckhausen's need achievement measure. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan at Ann Arbor.Google Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C. (2001b). An information processing account of implicit motive arousal. In Maehr, M. L. & Pintrich, P. (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement: Vol. 12. New directions in measures and methods (pp. 1–41). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C. (2007). A biobehavioral model of implicit power motivation: Arousal, reward and frustration. In Harmon-Jones, E. & Winkielman, P. (Eds.), Fundamentals of social neuroscience (pp. 176–196). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Goal imagery: Bridging the gap between implicit motives and explicit goals. Journal of Personality, 67, 1–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2001). Assessment of implicit motives with a research version of the TAT: Picture profiles, gender differences and relations to other personality measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77, 71–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2002). Inhibited power motivation and persuasive communication: A lens model analysis. Journal of Personality, 70, 553–582.Google ScholarPubMed
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2005). An implicit motive perspective on competence. In Elliot, A. J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 31–51). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C., Campbell, K. L., & McClelland, D. C. (1999). Implicit power motivation moderates men's testosterone responses to imagined and real dominance success. Hormones and Behavior, 36, 234–241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultheiss, O. C., Pang, J. S., Torges, C. M., Wirth, M. M., & Treynor, W. (2005). Perceived facial expressions of emotion as motivational incentives: Evidence from a differential implicit learning paradigm. Emotion, 5(1), 41–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultheiss, O. C., & Rohde, W. (2002). Implicit power motivation predicts men's testosteron changes and implicit learning in a contest situation. Hormones and Behavior, 41, 195–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C., Wirth, M. M., Torges, C. M., Pang, J. S., Villacorta, M. A., & Welsh, K. M. (2005). Effects of implicit power motivation on men's and women's implicit learning and testosterone changes after social victory or defeat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(1), 174–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80(1), 1–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275, 1593–1599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, R., & Curnow, C. (1988). Peak performance and age among superathletes: Track and field, swimming, baseball, tennis and golf. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 43, 113–120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (1996). A life-span model of successful aging. American Psychologist, 51, 702–714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, R., Wrosch, C., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). The life-span theory of control: Issues and evidence. In Zarit, S., Pearlin, L., & Schaie, K. W. (Eds.), Personal control in social and life course contexts: Societal impact on aging (pp. 233–262). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Schunk, D. H. (1982). Effects of effort attributional feedback on children's perceived self-efficacy and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 548–556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schur, W., & Weick, G. (1999). Wahnsinnskarriere [Mind-blowing career]. Frankfurt, Germany: Eichhorn.Google Scholar
Schuster, B., Ruble, D. N., & Weinert, F. E. (1998). Causal inferences and the positivity bias in children: The role of the covariation principle. Child Development, 69, 1577–1596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B. (1974). On going back to nature. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 183–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N. (1990). Feeling as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 527–561). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schwarzer, R. (1994). Kausalattributionen als gesundheitsbezogene Kognitionen [Causal attributions as health-related cognitions]. In Försterling, F. & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (Ed.), Attributionstheorie. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1982). Selbstwertdienliche Attributionen nach Leistungsrückmeldungen [Self-serving attri butions after performance feedback]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 14, 47–57.Google Scholar
Gallo, Schweiger I., Achtziger, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2003, September). Vorsatzeffekte bei der Kontrolle von Emotionen [Effects of implementation intentions in the control of emotions]. Paper presented at the Fachtagung Sozialpsychologie, Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Gallo, Schweiger I., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2004, April). Die vorsatzgesteuerte Inhibition der Emotion Ekel [Inhibiting the emotion of disgust by means of implementation intentions]. Paper presented at the symposium “Recent developments in research on implementation intentions” at the 46th meeting of the Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen (TeaP), Giessen, Germany.Google Scholar
Gallo, Schweiger I., Keil, A., Culloch, Mc K. C., Rockstroh, B., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (forthcoming). Strategic Automation of Emotion Control.
Scott, W. A. (1956). The avoidance of threatening material in imaginative behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 338–346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sears, R. R. (1950). Personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 1, 105–118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child rearing.Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.Google Scholar
Sechenow, I. (1863). The reflexes of the brain. Medizinsky Vestnik. In I. Sechenow, Selected works (pp. 263–336). Amsterdam: Bonset, 1968.
Sedikides, C., & Strube, M. J. (1997). Self-evaluation: To thine own self be good, to thine own self be sure, to thine own self be true, and to thine own self be better. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 209–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidenstücker, G., & Seidenstücker, E. (1974). Contribution to a computer evaluation of the Thematic Achievement Motivation Test by Heckhausen. Psychologische Beiträge, 16, 68–92.Google Scholar
Seifert, A. (2001). Fairness leicht gemacht: Zur Rolle von Diskrepanzprozessen in der vorsatzgesteuerten sozialen Urteilsbildung [How to be fair: The role of discrepancy processes and implementation intentions with regard to social judgments]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Konstanz, Germany.Google Scholar
Seligman, M., & Maier, S. F. (1967). Failure to escape traumatic shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(1), 1–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seligman, M. E. P. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review, 77, 406–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2, 307–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P., Abramson, L. Y., Semmel, A., & Baeyer, C. (1979). Depressive attributional style. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 242–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selz, O. (1910). Die experimentelle Untersuchung des Willensaktes [The experimental study of the volitional act]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 57, 241–270.Google Scholar
Selz, O. (1913). Über die Gesetze des geordneten Denkverlaufs [On the laws of ordered thought processes]. Stuttgart, Germany: Spemann.Google Scholar
Semmer, N. (1995). Die Komplexität der Motivation [The complexity of motivation]. Psychoscope, 16(19), 11–15.Google Scholar
Settersten, R. A., & Hagestad, G. (1996). What's the latest? Cultural age deadlines for family transition. Gerontologist, 36, 178–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seward, J. P. (1942). Note on the externalization of drive. Psychological Review, 49, 197–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seward, J. P. (1951). Experimental evidence for the motivating function of reward. Psychological Bulletin, 48, 130–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Forgetting all else: On the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1261–1280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, J. Y., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2000). The structure and substance of intrinsic motivation. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 105–127). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shah, J. Y., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). When opportunity knocks: Bottom-up priming of goals by means and the effects on self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1109–1122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaklee, H. (1983). Causal schemata: Description or explanation of judgment processes? A reply to Fiedler. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1010–1012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Implementation intentions and repeated behavior: Augmenting the predictive validity of the theory of planned behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 349–369.3.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (2000). Using implementation intentions to increase attendance for cervical cancer screening. Health Psychology, 19, 283–289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheeran, P., Webb, T. L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). The interplay between goals and implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 87–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheffield, F. D., & Campbell, B. A. (1954). The role of experience in the “spontaneous” activity of hungry rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 97–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheffield, F. D., & Roby, T. B. (1950). Reward value of non-nutrive sweet taste. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 471–481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheffield, F. D., Roby, T. B., & Campbell, B. A. (1954). Drive reduction versus consummatory behavior as determinants of reinforcement. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 349–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheffield, F. D., Wulff, J. J., & Backer, R. (1951). Reward value of copulation without sex drive reduction. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 44, 3–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 482–497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheldon, K. M., Elliot, A. J., Kim, Y., & Kasser, T. (2001). What is satisfying about satisfying events? Testing 10 candidate psychological needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 325–339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherrington, C. S. (1906). The integrative action of the nervous system.New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Shipley, T. E., & Veroff, J. (1952). A projective measure of need for affiliation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, 349–356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Short, J. -A. C., & Sorrentino, R. M. (1986). Achievement, affiliation, and group incentives: A test of the overmotivation hypothesis. Motivation and Emotion, 10, 115–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Showers, C. J., & Kling, K. C. (1996). Organization of self-knowledge: Implications for recovery from sad mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 578–590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siebert, T., & Vester, T. (1990). Zur Anreizstruktur des Musizierens: Motivationsanalyse einer Tätigkeit [The incentive structure of playing a musical instrument]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (2002). Variability and infant development [Special issue: Variability in infancy]. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 550–557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simmons, R. (1924). The relative effectiveness of certain incentives in animal learning. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 2(Serial no. 7).Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of man.New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Singh, D. (1970). Preference for bar-pressing to obtain reward over freeloading in rats and children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 73, 320–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, S. (1979). Relationships among projective and direct verbal measures of achievement motivation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 43, 45–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1935). Two types of a conditional reflex and a pseudotype. Journal of General Psychology, 12, 66–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental approach.New York: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior.New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1968). The technology of teaching.New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Skinner, E. A. (1990). Age differences in dimensions of perceived control during middle childhood: Implications for developmental conceptualizations and research. Child Development, 61, 1882–1890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 549–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, E. A., Chapman, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1988). Control, means-ends, and agency beliefs: A new conceptualization and its measurement during childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 117–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slavin, R. E. (1995). Cooperative learning (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Smith, A. A. (1953). An electromyographic study of tension in interrupted and completed tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 32–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. P. (Ed.). (1969). The origin and expression of achievement-related motives in children (pp. 102–150). New York: Sage.Google Scholar
Smith, C. P. (Ed.). (1992). Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis.New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. P., & Feld, S. C. (1958). How to learn the method of content analysis for n achievement, n affiliation, and n power. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 685–818). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Smith, D. A. (1971). Lateral hypothalamic stimulation: Experience and deprivation as a factors in rat's licking of empty drinking tubes. Psychological Science, 23, 329–331.Google Scholar
Smith, R. E. (2003). The costs of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: Investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 347–361.Google Scholar
Smock, C. D. (1957). Recall of interrupted and non-interrupted tasks as a function of experimentally induces anxiety and motivational relevance of the task stimuli. Journal of Personality, 25, 589–599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokolov, E. N. (1958). Vospriiate i uslovny refleks.Moscow, Russia: University of Moscow Press.Google Scholar
Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex.New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Sokolowksi, K. (1993). Emotion und Volition [Emotion and volition]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, K. (1986). Kognitionen und Emotionen in anschlussthematischen Situationen [Cognitions and emotions in affiliative situations]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wuppertal, Germany.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, K. (1992). Entwicklung eines Verfahrens zur Messung des Anschlussmotivs [Development of a technique to measure the affiliation motive]. Diagnostica, 38, 1–17.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, K. (1997). Sequentielle und imperative Konzepte des Willens [Sequential and imperative theories of volition]. Psychologische Beiträge, 39, 346–369.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, K. (2002). Emotion. In Prinz, W. & Müsseler, J. (Ed.), Allgemeine Psychologie (pp. 337–384). Heidelberg, Germany: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, K., & Kehr, H. M. (1999). Zum differentiellen Einfluss von Motiven auf die Wirkungen von Führungstrainings (MbO) [Differential impact of motive dispositions on the effects of management trainings (MbO)]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 20, 192–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokolowski, K., & Schmalt, H.-D. (1996). Emotionale und motivationale Einflussfaktoren in einer anschlussthematischen Konfliktsituation [Emotional and motivational influences in an affiliation-related conflict situation]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 18, 461–482.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, K., Schmalt, H.-D., Langens, T., & Puca, R. M. (2000). Assessing achievement, affiliation, and power motives all at once: The Multi-Motive-Grid (MMG). Journal of Personality Assessment, 74, 126–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokolowski, K., Schmitt, S., Jörg, J., & Ringendahl, H. (1997). Anschlussmotiv und Dopamin: Ein Vergleich zwischen Parkinson- und Rheumaerkrankten anhand implizit und explizit gemessener Motive [Implicit versus explicit affiliation motives and dopamine concentration: A comparison of parkinsonian and arthritis patients]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 18, 251– 259.Google Scholar
Solomon, R. L., & Wynne, L. C. (1953). Traumatic avoidance learning: Acquisition in normal dogs. Psychological Monographs, 67(Whole no. 354).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
S⊘rensen, A. B. (1986). Social structure and mechanisms of life-course processes. Human development and the life course: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 177–197). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., Hanna, S. E., & Roney, C. J. R. (1992). Uncertainty orientation. In Smith, C. P. (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 428–439). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., & Hewitt, E. C. (1984). The uncertainty-reducing properties of achievement tasks revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 884–899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., Roney, C. J. E., & Hewitt, E. C. (1988). Information value versus affective value and achievement behavior. In Halisch, F. & Bercken, J. H. L. (Eds.), International perspectives on achievement and task motivation.Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., & Sheppard, B. H. (1978). Effects of affiliation-related motives on swimmers in individual versus group competition: A field experiment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 704–714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., & Short, J.-A. (1977). The case of the mysterious moderates: Why motives sometimes fail to predict behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 478–484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., Short, J. C., & Raynor, J. O. (1984). Uncertainty orientation: Implications for affective and cognitive views of achievement behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 189–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spangler, G., Bräutigam, I., & Stadler, R. (1984). Handlungsentwicklung in der frühen Kindheit und ihre Abhängigkeit von der kognitiven Entwicklung und der emotionalen Erregbarkeit des Kindes [Behavioral development in early childhood and its dependence on the cognitive development and emotional excitability of the child]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 16, 181–193.Google Scholar
Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement: Two meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 140–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spangler, W. D., & House, R. J. (1991). Presidential effectiveness and the leadership motive profile. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 439–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity and femininity.Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1956). Behavior theory and conditioning.New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1960). Behavior theory and learning: Selected papers.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, K. W., Farber, T. E., & McFann, H. H. (1956). The relation of anxiety (drive) level to performance in competitional and noncompetitional paired-associates. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52, 296–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, K. W., & Runquist, W. N. (1958). Temporal effects of conditioned fear on the eyelid reflex. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 613–616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinath, B., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2003). Goal orientation and achievement: The role of ability self-concept and failure perception. Learning and Instruction, 13, 403–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spirito, A., & Hartford, K. (1990). Social skills and depression in adolescent suicide attempters. Adolescence, 25, 543–552.Google ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1996). Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93(24), 13515–13522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A. (1977). Weariness of strangers and the study of infant development. Child Development, 48, 731–746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1979). The coherence of individual development: Early care, attachment and subsequent developmental issues. American Psychologist, 34, 834–841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). The ontogenesis of smiling and laughter: A perspective on the organization of development in infancy. Psychological Review, 83, 173–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stahl, M. J. (1983). Achievement, power, and managerial motivation: Selecting managerial talent with the job choice experience. Personnel Psychology, 36, 775–789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stayton, D. F., Hogan, R., & Ainsworth, M. D. (1971). Infant obedience and maternal behavior: The origins of socialization reconsidered. Child Development, 42(4), 1057–1069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steele, R. S. (1977). Power motivation, activation, and inspirational speeches. Journal of Personality, 45, 53–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steiner, M. (2006). Motivationale Kompetenz und Anreize im Badminton [Motivational competence and intentives in badminton]. Unpublished Lizensiat thesis, University of Zürich, Switzerland.Google Scholar
Steller, B. (1992). Vorsätze und die Wahrnehmung günstiger Gelegenheiten [Intentions and grasping favorable opportunities]. Munich, Germany: tuduv.Google Scholar
Steller, B. (1992). Vorsätze und die Wahrnehmung günstiger Gelegenheiten [Implementation intentions and the perception of viable opportunities to act]. Tuduv: Munich, Germany.Google Scholar
Stern, W. (1935). Allgemeine Psychologie auf personalistischerGrundlage [General psychology: From the personalistic standpoint]. The Hague, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2003). WICS: A model of leadership in organizations. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 2, 386–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, L., & Jones, E. E. (1976). Defensive attribution and the Lelley cube. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 809–820.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, A. J., & Chester, N. L. (1982). Sex differences in human social motives: Achievement, affiliation, and power. In Stewart, A. J. (Ed.), Motivation and society (pp. 172–218). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier, J., Kammer, D., Pelster, A., & Niketta, R. (1985). Attributionsstil und Bewertung als Risikofaktoren der depressiven Reaktion [Attribution style and evaluation as risk factors for depressive symptoms]. Diagnostica, 31, 300–311.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1988). Erlernte Hilflosigkeit, Handlungskontrolle und Leistung [Learned helplessness, action control, and achievement]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1989). Attributional style and depressive mood reactions. Journal of Personality, 57, 581–599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1994). Choice of decision-making strategies and action versus state orientation. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality, action versus state orientation (pp. 167–176). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1995). Eine attributionale Analyse elterlichen Erziehungsverhaltens [An attributional analysis of parenting behavior]. In Pawlik, K. (Ed.), Bericht über den 39. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie(Vol. 2, pp. 445–450). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2004). Die Suche nach und das Nachdenken über Ursachen [Searching for and reflecting on causes]. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Giessen, Germany.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Grüner, S. (2005). Reattributionsstraining: Ein Leitfaden [Reattribution training: A guide]. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Giessen, Germany.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Assimi, S. (2002). Attributionale Analyse aggressiven Verhaltens bei Jungen und Mädchen [Attribu tional analysis of aggressive behavior in boys and girls]. In Baumann, M., Keinath, A., & Krems, J. F. (Eds.), Experimentelle Psychologie (p. 65). Regensburg, Germany: Roderer.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., Balke, S., & Schlangen, B. (1996). Lern- versus Leistungszielorientierung als Bedingungen des Lernfortschritts [Learning goal orientation vs. performance goal orientation as determinants of learning progress]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 28, 169–187.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Gerlach, H. (1997). Aggressives Verhalten bei Kindern und Jugendlichen aus attributionstheoretischer Sicht [Aggressive behavior of children and adolescents from an attributional point of view]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 11, 203–209.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Grüner, S. (2005). Reattributionsstraining: Ein Leitfaden [Reattribution training: A guide]. Manuscript submitted for pub lication.
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., Kammer, D., & Adolphs, J. (1988). Attributionsstil und Bewertung bei depressiven versus nichtdepressiven Patienten [Attribution style and evaluation in depressive versus nondepressive patients]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, 17, 46–54.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., Reisenzein, R., & Martini, A. (1995). The role of surprise in the attribution process. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 5–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Rheinberg, F. (Eds.). (2003). Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept [Diagnosis of motivation and self-concept]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Schürmann, M. (1991). Attributionsstil als Risikofaktor der depressiven Reaktion bei Kindern [Attribution style as a risk factor for depressive symptoms in children]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 23, 318– 329.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., Schürmann, M., Eckert, C., & Pelster, A. (1994). Attributionsstil-Fragebogen für Kinder und Jugendliche (ASF-KJ) [Attribution style questionnaire for children and adolescents (ASF-KJ)]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Stipek, D. (1996). Motivation and instruction. In Berliner, D. C. & Calfee, R. C. (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 85–113). New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Stipek, D., & Daniels, D. (1990). Children's use of dispositional attributions in predicting the performance and behavior of classmates. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 11, 13–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stipek, D., & Gralinski, J. H. (1996). Children's beliefs about intelligence and school performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 397–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stipek, D., & Hoffman, J. (1980). Development of children's performance-related judgements. Child Development, 51, 912–914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stipek, D., & Kowalski, P. (1989). Learned helplessness in task-orienting versus performance-orienting testing conditions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3), 384–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stipek, D., Recchia, S., & McClintic, S. (1992). Self-evaluation in young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57(1).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stipek, D., & Tannatt, L. (1984). Children's judgments of their own and peers' academic competence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 75–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stipek, D. J., & Decotis, K. (1988). Children's understanding of the implications of causal attributions for emotional experiences. Child Development, 59, 1601–1616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, P. J., Dumphy, D. C., Smith, M. S., & Ogilvie, D. M. (1966). The general inquirer.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stricker, E. M., & Verbalis, J. G. (2002). Hormones and ingestive behaviors. In Becker, J. B., Breedlove, S. M., & Crews, D. (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinology (2nd ed., pp. 451–473). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stroebe, W., Eagly, A. H., & Stroebe, M. S. (1977). Friendly or just polite? The effect of self-esteem on attributions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 7, 265– 274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stuchlikova, I., & Man, F. (1999). Motivational structure of state and action oriented alcoholics. Studia Psychologica, 41, 63–72.Google Scholar
Stumpf, H., Angleitner, A., Wieck, T., Jackson, D. N., & Beloch-Till, H. (1985). Deutsche Personality Research Form (PRF) [German Personality Research Form (PRF)]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Stutz, A. M., Morrison, C. D., & Argyropoulos, G. (2005). The agouti-related protein and its role in energy homeostasis. Peptides, 26(10), 1771–1781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sueton Tranquilinas. De vita Caesarum.
Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry.New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Sullivan, R. M., Wilson, D. A., Wong, R., Correa, A., & Leon, M. (1990). Modified behavioral and olfactory bulb responses to maternal odors in preweanling rats. Brain Research. Developmental Brain Research, 53(2), 243–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surber, C. F. (1980). The development of reversible operations in judgments of ability, effort and performance. Child Development, 51, 1018–1029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutherland, R. W., & Rudy, J. W. (1989). Configurational association theory: The role of hippocampal formation in learning, memory and amnesia. Psychobiology, 17, 129–144.Google Scholar
Sutton, S. K., & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Prefrontal brain asymmetry: A biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems. Psychological Science, 8, 204–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svenson, G. R., Oestergren, P.-O., Merlo, J., & Rastam, L. (2002). Action control and situational risks in the prevention of risks HIV and STIs: Individual, dyadic, and social influences on consistent condom use in a university population. AIDS Education and Prevention, 14, 515–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swithers, S. E., & Martinson, F. A. (1998). Habituation of oral responding in adult rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112(1), 213–224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taira, K., & Rolls, E. T. (1996). Receiving grooming as a reinforcer for the monkey. Physiology & Behavior, 59(6), 1189–1192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taub, E., & Berman, A. J. (1968). Movement and learning in the absence of sensory feedback. In Freedman, S. J. (Ed.), The neuropsychology of spatially oriented behavior (pp. 173–192). Homewood, IL: Dorsey.Google Scholar
Tauer, J. M., & Harackiewicz, (1999). Winning isn't everything: Competition, achievement orientation, intrinsic motivation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 209–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, J. A. (1953). A personality scale of manifest anxiety. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48, 285–290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, J. A., & Spence, K. W. (1952). The relationship of anxiety level to performance in serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 61–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social-psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. (1994). Positive illusions and well-being revisited: Separating fact from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 21–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. E., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1995). Effects of mindset on positive illusions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 213–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. E., Neter, E., & Wayment, H. A. (1995). Self-evaluation processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1278–1287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teachman, J. D., & Heckhert, A. (1985). The impact of age and children on remarriage. Journal of Family Issues, 6, 185–203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teevan, R. C., & McGhee, P. E. (1972). Childhood development of the fear of failure motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 345–348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terhune, K. W. (1968a). Motives, situation, and interpersonal conflict within Prisoner's Dilemma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Monograph Supplement 8, 1–24.Google Scholar
Terhune, K. W. (1968b). Studies of motives, cooperation, and conflict within laboratory microcosms. Buffalo Studies, 4 (1), 29–58.Google Scholar
The Oxford English Dictionary (2007). Volume IX. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Thierry, D. (2004). Financial compensation at work: A motivational mess? In Wegge, J. & Schmidt, K.-H. (Eds.), Förderung von Arbeitsmotivation und Gesundheit in Organisationen. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Thomae, H. (Ed.). (1965). Handbuch der Psychologie. Allgemeine Psychologie II: Motivation [Handbook of psychology. Germany psychology II: Motivation]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Thomas, E. A. C. (1983). Notes on effort and achievement oriented behavior. Psychological Review, 90, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R. (1987). Development of children's inferences of the emotions of others. Developmental Psychology, 22, 124–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R., Gupta, S., Miller, K., Mills, S., & Orr, S. (2004). The effects of vasopressin on human facial responses related to social communication. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 35–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of associative processes in animals. Psychological Review, Monographs Supplement 5, 551–553.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1913). Educational psychology. New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Implicit and self-attributed achievement motives: Concordance and predictive validity. Journal of Personality, 70, 729–755.Google ScholarPubMed
Thurstone, L. L. (1937). Ability, motivation, and speed. Psychometrika, 2, 249–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiedens, L. Z., & Fragale, A. R. (2003). Complementarity in dominant and submissive nonverbal behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 558–568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct.London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Toates, F. M. (1981). The control of ingestive behavior by internal and external stimuli: A theoretical review. Appetite, 2, 35–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toates, F. M. (1986). Motivational systems. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1926). The nature of fundamental drives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 20, 349–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men.New York: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1951). A psychological model. In Parsons, T. & Shils, E. (Eds.), Toward a general theory of action (pp. 279–361). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1952). A cognition motivation model. Psychological Review, 59, 389–400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolman, E. C. (1959). Principles of purposive behavior. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. II, pp. 92–157). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Toman, W. (1960). On the periodicity of motivation. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1960 (pp. 80–96). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Tomasik, M. J. (2003). Adjusting goal aspirations when getting close to a developmental deadline: The role of primary and secondary control strategies. Unpublished Diplom thesis, Free University of Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, and consciousness. Vol. 1. The positive affects. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S. (1970). Affect as the primary motivational system. In Arnold, M. (Ed.), Feelings and emotions (pp.101–111). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S. (1981). The quest for primary motives: Biography and autobiography of an idea. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 306–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1977). Descriptive analyses of infant communicative behavior. In Schaffer, H. R. (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp. 227–270). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1980). The foundations of intersubjectivity: Development of interpersonal and cooperative understanding in infants. The social foundations of language and thought: Essays in honor of J. S. Bruner. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: McLeod.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 3–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding, and acts of meaning in the first year. Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 183–229). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1972). The analysis of subjective culture. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1997). Cross-cultural perspectives on personality. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 440–464). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Triemer, A. (2001). Ambulantes psychophysiologisches 24-Stunden-Monitoring zur Erfassung von arbeitsbezogenen Stimmungen und Emotionen [Ambulant psychophysiological 24-hour monitoring to assess work-related mood and emotion]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Technical University of Dresden, Germany.Google Scholar
Triemer, A., & Rau, R. (2001). Stimmungskurven im Arbeitsalltag – eine Feldstudie [Mood curves at normal working days – A field study]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 22, 42–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. (1975). Seeking information about one's own ability as a determinant of choice among tasks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 1004–1013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. (1980). Self-assessment, self-enhancement, and task preference. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 116–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. (1983). Self-assessment in achievement behavior. In Suls, J. M. & Greenwald, A. G. (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 2, pp. 93–121). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Trope, Y. (1986a). Identification and inferential processes in dispositional attribution. Psychological Review, 93, 239–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. (1986b). Testing self-enhancement and self-enhancement theories of achievement motivation: A reply to Sohn's critique. Motivation and Emotion, 10, 247–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. (1986c). Self-enhancement and self-assessment in achievement behavior. In Sorrentino, R. M. & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 350–378). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Trope, Y., & Brickman, P. (1975). Difficulty and diagnosticity as determinants of choice among tasks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 918–926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trötschel, R., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2004). Verhandlungsführung – Psychologische Grundlagen [Negotiation – Psychological fundaments]. In Sommer, G. & Fuchs, A. (Eds.), Lehrbuch Konflikt- und Friedenspsychologie (pp. 158–177). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz, PVU.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C. (1975). Häusliche Umwelt und Motiventwicklung [Home environment and motivation]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C. (1982a). The development of achievement motivation and individual differences: Ecological determinants. In Hartup, W. W. (Ed.), Review of child development research (Vol. 6, pp. 669–703). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C. (1982b). Der ökologische Ansatz in der Erforschung der Leistungsmotivgenese [The ecological approach in research on the development of the achievement motive]. In Vaskowics, L. (Ed.), Umweltbedingungen familialer Sozialisation (pp. 168–203). Stuttgart, Germany: Enke.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C. (1987). The role of toys and games in an ecological approach to motive development. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 179–199). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trudewind, C. (2000). Curiosity and anxiety as motivational determinants of cognitive development. In Heckhausen, J. (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 15–38). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C., Brünger, T., & Krieger, K. (1986). Parental expectations and the development of achievement motivation. In Bercken, J. H., DeBruyn, E., & Bergen, T. (Eds.), Achievement and task motivation (pp. 179–200). Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C., & Husarek, B. (1979). Mutter-Kind-Interaktion bei der Hausaufgabenbetreuung und die Leistungsmotiventwicklung im Grundschulalter: Analyse einer ökologischen Schlüsselsituation [Mother-child interaction in homework supervision and the development of the achievement motive in primary school age: Analysis of a key ecological situation]. In Walter, H. & Oerter, R. (Eds.), Ökologie und Entwicklung (pp. 229–246). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C., & Schneider, K. (1994). Individual differences in the development of exploratory behavior: Methodological considerations. In Keller, H., Schneider, K., & Henderson, B. (Eds.), Curiosity and exploration (pp. 151–176). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trudewind, C., Unzner, L., & Schneider, K. (1997). Die Entwicklung der Leistungsmotivation [The development of achievement motivation]. In Keller, H. (Ed.), Handbuch der Kleinkindforschung (pp. 587–622). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C., & Windel, A. (1991). Elterliche Einflussnahme auf die kindliche Kompetenzentwicklung: Schulleistungsentwicklung und ihre motivationale Vermittlung [Parental influence on the development of child competence: Development of school achievement and its motivational antecedents]. In Pekrun, R. & Fend, H. (Eds.), Schule und Persönlichkeitsentwicklung (pp. 131–148). Stuttgart, Germany: Enke.Google Scholar
Tucker, D. M., & Williamson, P. A. (1984). Asymmetric neural control systems in human self-regulation. Psychological Re view, 91, 185–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuerlinckx, F., Boeck, P., & Lens, W. (2002). Measuring needs with the Thematic Apperception Test: A psychometric study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 448–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there?American Psychologist, 40, 495–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tupes, E. C., & Christal, R. C. (1992). Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings. Journal of Personality, 60, 225–252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tweer, R. (1976). Das Ökonomieprinzip in der Anstrengungskalkulation: Eine entwicklungspsychologische Untersuchung [The parsimony principle in the calculation of effort: An investigation from the perspective of developmental psychology]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2001). Age and birth cohort differences in self-esteem: A cross temporal meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 321–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Schonen, S., Crivello, F., Reutter, B., Aujard, Y., & Mazoyer, B. (2002). Neural correlates of woman face processing by 2-month-old infants. NeuroImage, 15, 454–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uhlenberg, P. (1974). Cohort variations in family life cycle experiences of U.S. females. Journal of Marriage and Family, 36, 284–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uleman, J. S. (1966). A new TAT measure of the need for power. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Uleman, J. S. (1971). Dyadic influence in an “ESP study” and TAT measures of the needs for influence and power. Journal of Personality Assessment, 35, 248–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uleman, J. S. (1972). The need for influence: Development and validation of a measure, and comparison with the need for power. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 85, 157–214.Google Scholar
Undeutsch, U., & Hermans, H. (1976). Leistungsmotivationstest für Jugendliche (LMT-J) [Achievement motivation test for adolescents]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Ungless, M. A. (2004). Dopamine: the salient issue. Trends in Neuroscience, 27, 702–706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uvnaes-Moberg, K. (1998). Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 819–835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentine, C. W. (1930). The innate bases of fear. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 37, 394–419.Google Scholar
Lawick-Goodall, J. (1968). The behavior of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Area. Animal Behavior Monographs, 1, 161–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1975). The behavior of the chimpanzee. In Kurth, G. & Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (Eds.), Hominisation und Verhalten. Stuttgart, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Overwalle, F., & Heylighen, F. (1995). Relating covariation information to causal dimensions through principles of contrast and invariance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 435–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veroff, J. (1957). Development and validation of a projective measure of power motivation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veroff, J. (1969). Social comparison and the development of achievement motivation. In Smith, C. P. (Ed.), Achievement-related motives in children (pp. 46–101). New York: Sage.Google Scholar
Veroff, J. (1982). Assertive motivations: Achievement versus power. In Winter, D. G. & Stewart, A. J. (Eds.), Motivation and society. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Veroff, J. (1992). Power motivation. In Smith, C. P. (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 278–285). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veroff, J., Atkinson, J. W., Feld, S. C., & Gurin, G. (1960). The use of thematic apperception to assess motivation in a nationwide interview study. Psychological Monographs, 74 (12, Whole no. 499).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veroff, J., Depner, C., Kulka, R., & Douvan, E. (1980). Comparison of American motives: 1957 versus 1976. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1249–1262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veroff, J., & Feld, S. C. (1970). Marriage and work in America. New York: Van Nostrand-Reinhold.Google Scholar
Veroff, J., Reuman, D., & Feld, S. C. (1984). Motives in American men and women across the adult life span. Developmental Psychology, 20, 1142–1158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veroff, J., & Veroff, J. B. (1972). Reconsideration of a measure of power motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 78, 279–291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volling, B. L., McElwain, N. L., Notaro, P. C., & Herrera, C. U. (2002). Parents' emotional availability and infant emotional competence: Predictors of parent-infant attachment and emerging self-regulation. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 447–465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vollmeyer, R., & Rheinberg, F. (2003, August). Task difficulty and flow. Paper presented at the biennial EARLI conference, Padova, Italy.Google Scholar
Vontobel, J. (1970). Leistungsbedürfnis und soziale Umwelt [Need for achievement and the social environment]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation.New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Vuilleumier, P., Richardson, M. P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7 (11), 1271–1278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vygotski, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1957). The strategy of the genes.London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Wadsworth, M., &. Ford, D. (1983). Assessment of personal goal hierarchies. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 30, 514–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. (1969). Levels of symbolization in adolescent adjustment patterns. International Journal of Symbology, 1 (1), 67–74.Google Scholar
Wahba, M. A., & House, R. J. (1974). Expectancy theory in work and motivation: Some logical and methodological issues. Human Relations, 27, 121–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wainer, H. A., & Rubin, I. M. (1971). Motivation of research and development entrepreneurs: Determinants of company success. In Kolb, D. A., Rubin, I. M., & McIntire, J. (Eds.), Organizational psychology (pp. 131–139). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Walker, E. L. (1969). Reinforcement – The one ring. In Trapp, J. T. (Ed.), Reinforcement and behavior (pp. 47–62). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Walker, E. L. (1973). Psychological complexity and preference: A hedgehog theory of behavior. In Berlyne, D. E. & Madsen, K. B. (Eds.), Pleasure, reward, preference (pp. 65–97). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Walker, E. L., & Heyns, R. W. (1962). Conformity and conflict of needs. In Walter, E. L. & Heyns, R. W. (Eds.), Anatomy for conformity (pp. 54–68). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Wallen, K. (2001). Sex and context: Hormones and primate sexual motivation. Hormones and Behavior, 40 (2), 339–357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walschburger, P. (1994). Action control and excessive demand: Effects of situational and personality factors on psychological and physiological functions during stressful transactions. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (eds.), Volition and personality (pp. 233–266). Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.Google Scholar
Walster, E. (1964). The temporal sequence of post-decision processes. In Festinger, L. (Ed.), Conflict, decision, and dissonance (pp. 112–127). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Warden, C. J., Jenkins, T. N., & Warner, L. H. (1936). Comparative psychology. New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
Wasna, M. (1970). Die Entwicklung der Leistungsmotivation [The development of achievement motivation]. Munich, Germany: Reinhardt.Google Scholar
Watson, D. (1982). The actor and the observer: How are their perceptions of causality divergent?Psychological Bulletin, 92, 682–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Extraversion and its positive emotional core. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 767–793). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 820–838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1919). Psychology from the standpoint of a behaviorist.Philadelphia: Lippincott.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1924). Behaviorism.New York: People's Institute Company.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B., & Morgan, J. J. B. (1917). Emotional reactions and psychological experimentation. American Journal of Psychology, 28, 163–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. S. (1966). The development and generalization of contingency awareness in early infancy: Some hypotheses. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 12, 123–135.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S. (1972). Smiling, cooing, and “The Game.”Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 323–339.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S., & Ramey, C. (1972). Reactions to response contingent stimulation in early infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 219–228.Google Scholar
Watt, H. J. (1905). Experimentelle Beiträge zu einer Theorie des Denkens [Experimental contributions to a theory of thought]. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 4, 289–436.Google Scholar
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Can implementation intentions help to overcome ego-depletion?Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 279–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber, M. (1904). Die protestantische Ethik und der Geist des Kapitalismus [The Protestant ethic and the spirit of capitalism]. Archiv für Sozialwissenschaft und Sozialpolitik, 20, 1–54.Google Scholar
Weber, M. (1921). III. Abteilung. Wirtschaft und Gesellschaft. 1. Die Wirtschaft und die gesellschaftlichen Ordnungen und Mächte. Grundriss der Sozialökonomik [Economy and society. 1. The economy and social powers and structures] (New ed. 1964). Tübingen, Germany: Mohr-Siebeck.Google Scholar
Weber, M. (1947). The theory of social and economic organization. The theory of social and economic organization [1st American Edition (A. M. Henderson & T. Parsons, Trans.)]. Oxford University Press: New York, NY.Google Scholar
Wegge, J., Quaeck, A., & Kleinbeck, U. (1996). Zur Faszinationskraft von Video- und Computerspielen bei Studenten: Welche Motive befriedigen die “bunte Welt am Draht”? [The fascination of video and computer games among students]. In Bräuer, K. & Kittler, U. (Eds.), Pädagogische Psychologie und ihre Anwendungen (Vol. 2, pp. 51–76). Essen, Germany: Die blaue Eule.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 35–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M., & Wheatley, T. (1999). Apparent mental causation: Sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist, 54, 480–492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberger, J., & McClelland, D. C. (1990). Cognitive versus traditional motivational models: Irreconcilable or complementary? In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 562–597). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Weinberger, J., & Silverman, L. H. (1987). Subliminal psychodynamic activation: A method for studying psychoanalytic dynamic propositions. In Hogan, R. & Jones, H. (Eds.), Perspective in personality (Vol. 2, pp. 251–287). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1965a). Need achievement and the resumption of incompleted tasks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 165–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1965b). The effects of unsatisfied achievement motivation on persistence and subsequent performance. Journal of Personality, 33, 428–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1966). Role of success and failure in the learning of easy and complex tasks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 339–344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, B. (1967). Implications of the current theory of achievement motivation for research and performance in the classroom. Psychology in the School, 4, 164–171.3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1970). New conceptions in the study of achievement motivation. In Maher, B. (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality research (Vol. 5, pp. 67–109). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1972). Theories of motivation.Chicago: Markham.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1974). Achievement motivation and attribution theory.Morristown, NJ: General Learning.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 3–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, B. (1980). A cognitive (attribution) – emotion – action model of motivated behavior: An analysis of judgments of help-giving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 186–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1985a). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548–573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1985b). “Spontaneous” causal thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 74–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion.New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1992). Human motivation. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1994). Sünde versus Krankheit [Sin versus illness]. In Försterling, F. & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (Eds.), Attributionspsychologie (pp. 1–25). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility. A foundation for a theory of social conduct. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., Frieze, I. H., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., & Rosenbaum, R. M. (1971). Perceiving the causes of success and failure.New York: General Learning.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., Heckhausen, H., Meyer, W.-U., & Cook, R. E. (1972). Causal ascriptions and achievement behavior: A conceptual analysis of effort and reanalysis of locus of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 239–248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, B., & Kukla, A. (1970). An attributional analysis of achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B., Kun, A., & Benesh-Weiner, M. (1980). The development of mastery, emotions, and morality from an attributional perspective. Minnesota Symposia on Child Development, 13, 103–129.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., & Peter, N. (1973). A cognitive-developmental analysis of achievement and moral judgments. Developmental Psychology, 9, 290–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B., & Schneider, K. (1971). Drive versus cognitive theory: A reply to Boor and Harmon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 258–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinert, A. B. (2001). Berufsorientierungstest [Career orientation test]. In Wottawa, H. & Sarges, W. (Eds.), Handbuch Wirtschaftspsychologischer Testverfahren (pp. 125–130). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst.Google Scholar
Weinert, F. E. (1991). Vorwort zur deutschsprachigen Ausgabe [Preface to the German edition]. In Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.), Die außergewöhnliche Erfahrung im Alltag: Die Psychologie des Flow-Erlebens (pp. 7–9). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Weisfeld, G. E., & Beresford, J. M. (1982). Erectedness of posture as an indicator of dominance or success in humans. Motivation and Emotion, 6, 113–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, A., King, J. E., & Enns, R. M. (2002). Subjective well-being is heritable and genetically correlated with dominance in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1141–1149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, A., King, J. E., & Figueredo, A. J. (2000). The heritability of personality factors in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavior Genetics, 30, 213–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisz, J. R. (1983). Can I control it? The pursuit of veridical answers across the life span. In Baltes, P. B. & Brim, O. G. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 3) (pp. 233–300). Orlando, Forida: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Weisz, J. R., Yeates, K. O., Robertson, D., & Beckmann, J. C. (1982). Perceived contingency of skill and chance events: A developmental analysis. Developmental Psychology, 18, 898–905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welker, W. L. (1956). Some determinants of play and exploration in chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 49, 84–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, G. L., & Harvey, J. H. (1977). Do people use consensus information in making causal attributions?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 279–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wendt, H. W. (1955). Motivation, effort, and performance. In McClelland, D. C. (Ed.), Studies in motivation (pp. 448–459). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Wendt, H. W. (1967). Verhaltensmodelle des Nichtwissenschaftlers: Einige biographische und Antriebskorrelate der wahrgenommenen Beziehung zwischen Erfolgswahrscheinlichkeit und Zielanreiz [Models of a nonscientist's behavior: Some biographical and motivational correlates of the perceived relationship between the probability of success and incentive value]. Psychologische Forschung, 30, 226–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wentzel, K. R. (1989). Adolescent classroom goals, standards for performance and academic achievement: An interactionist perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81 (2), 131–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wertlieb, D., Weigel, C., & Feldstein, M. (1987). Measuring children's coping. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57 (4), 548–560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, M. M., & Konner, M. J. (1976). The role of the father: An anthropological perspective.New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Westergaard, G. C., Suomi, S. J., Higley, J. D., & Mehlman, P. T. (1999). Csf 5-hiaa and aggression in female macaque monkeys: Species and interindividual differences. Psychopharmacology, 146 (4), 440–446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 331–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, G. M. (1980). Conceptual universals in interpersonal language. American Anthropologist, 82, 759–781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, R. W. (1960). Competence and the psychosexual stages of development. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1960 (pp. 97–140). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Wicklund, R. A., & Brehm, J. W. (1976). Perspectives on cognitive dissonance. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wicklund, R. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion.Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wicklund, R. A. (1986). Orientation to the environment versus preoccupation with human potential. In Sorrentino, R. M., & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 64–95). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wiese, B. S. (2000). Berufliche und familiäre Zielstrukturen [Occupational and family goal structures]. Münster, Germany: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Wiese, B. S., & Freund, A. M. (2000). The interplay of work and family in young and middle adulthood. In Heckhausen, J. (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 233–249). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Wiese, R. A., & Rompré, P. P. (1989). Brain dopamine and reward. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 191–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1997). Changes in children's competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elementary school years. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 451–469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S, Yoon, K. S., & Harold, R. D. (1997). Change in children's competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elementary school years: A 3-year study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 451–469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, D. R., & Williams, H. (1969). Auto-maintenance in the pigeon: Sustained pecking despite contingent non-reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 511–520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, G. C. (1957). Pleitropy, natural selection, and the evolution of senescence. Evolution, 11, 398–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, S. B. (1938). Resistance to extinction as a function of the number of reinforcements. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 506–521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1980). Sociobiology: The abridged edition. Cambridge, MA: Belknap(Harvard.Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive unconscious.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wine, J. (1971). Test anxiety and direction of attention. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 92–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wine, J. (1982). Evaluation anxiety: A cognitive-attentional construct. In Krohne, H. W. & Laux, L. C. (Eds.), Achievement, stress, and anxiety (pp. 207–219). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Wingfield, J. C., Hegner, R. E., Dufty, A. M., & Ball, G. F. (1990). The “challenge hypothesis”: Theoretical implications for patterns of testosterone secretion, mating systems, and breeding strategies. The American Naturalist, 136, 829–846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winslow, J. T., & Insel, T. R. (2002). The social deficits of the oxytocin knockout mouse. Neuropeptides, 36 (2–3), 221–229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, D. G. (1967). Power motivation in thought and action. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1973). The power motive. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1987a). Enhancement of an enemy's power motivation as a dynamic of conflict escalation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 41–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1987b). Leader appeal, leader performance, and the motive profiles of leaders and followers: A study of American presidents and elections. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 196–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1988). The power motive in women – and men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 510–519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1991a). Manual for scoring motive imagery in running text (3rd ed.). Unpublished scoring manual, University of Michigan at Ann Arbor.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1991b). Measuring personality at a distance: Development of an integrated system for scoring motives in running text. In Hogan, R. (Series Ed.) & Ozer, D., Healy, J. M., & Stewart, A. J. (Vol. Eds.), Perspectives in personality: Vol. 3B. Approaches to understanding lives (pp. 59–89). London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1993). Power, affiliation, and war: Three tests of a motivational model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 532–545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1996). Personality: Analysis and interpretation of lives.New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (2001). Measuring Bush's motives. Proceedings of the International Society for Political Psychology. ISPP News, 8–9.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G., & Barenbaum, N. B. (1985). Responsibility and the power motive in women and men. Journal of Personality, 53, 335–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D. G., John, O. P., Stewart, A. J., Klohnen, E. C., & Duncan, L. E. (1998). Traits and motives: Toward an integration of two traditions in personality research. Psychological Review, 105, 230–250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, D. G., & Stewart, A. J. (1977). Power motive reliability as a function of retest instructions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 436–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D. G., & Stewart, A. J. (1978). Power motivation. In London, H. & Exner, J. (Eds.), Dimensions of personality (pp. 391–447). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G., Stewart, A., John, O. P., Klohnen, E. C., & Duncan, L. E. (1998). Traits and motives: Toward an integration of two traditions in personality research. Psychological Review, 105, 230–250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, D. G., & Wiecking, F. A. (1971). The new Puritans: Achievement and power motives of New Left radicals. Behavioral Science, 16, 523–530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winterbottom, M. R. (1953). The relation of childhood training in independence to achievement motivation. Dissertation Abstracts, 13, 440–441.Google Scholar
Winterbottom, M. R. (1958). The relation of need for achievement to learning experiences in independence and mastery. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action and society (pp. 453–478). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Wirth, M. M., Welsh, K. M., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2006). Salivary cortisol changes in humans after winning or losing a dominance contest depend on implicit power motivation. Hormones and Behavior, 49 (3), 346–352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wise, R. A., & Rompré, P.-P. (1989). Brain dopamine and reward. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 191–225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witkowski, T., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1998). Performance deficits following failure: Learned helplessness or self-esteem protection?British Journal of Educational Psychology, 37, 59–71.Google ScholarPubMed
Wittling, W. (1990). Psychophysiological correlates of human brain asymmetry: Blood pressure changes during lateralized presentation of an emotionally laden film. Neuropsychologia, 28, 457–470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woike, B. A. (1995). Most memorable experiences: Evidence for a link between implicit and explicit motives and social cognitive processes in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1081–1091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woike, B., Gershkowich, I., Piorkowski, R., & Poco, M. (1999). The role of motives in the content and structure of autobiographical memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 600–612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woike, B., & Poco, M. (2001). Motive-related memories: Content, structure, and affect. Journal of Personality, 69, 391–415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolters, C. A. (2003). Regulation of motivation: Evaluating an underemphasized aspect of self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 189–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, D. J., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, R. E. (1986). Task complexity: Definition of the construct. Organizational Behavior and Decision Processes, 37, 60–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, R. E., & Locke, E. A. (1990). Goal setting and strategy effects on complex tasks. In Staw, B. & Cummings, L. (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 73–110). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Wood, R. E., Mento, A. J., & Locke, E. A. (1987). Task complexity as a moderator of goal effects: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 416–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodson, J. C. (2002). Including “learned sexuality” in the organization of sexual behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 69–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. (1918). Dynamic psychology.New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Worchel, P. (1957). Adaptability screening of flying personnel: Development of a self-concept inventory for predicting maladjustment. ASAF School of Aviation Medicine Report, No. 56–62.
Wright, R. (1994). The moral animal: Evolutionary psychology and everyday life.New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Wright, R. A. (1996). Brehm's theory of motivation as a model of effort and cardiovascular response. In Gollwitzer, P. M. & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 424–453). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wrosch, C., & Freund, A. (2001). Self-regulation of normative and non-normative developmental challenges. Human Development, 44 (5), 264–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrosch, C., & Heckhausen, J. (1999). Control processes before and after passing a developmental deadline: Activation and reactivation of intimate relationship goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 415–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrosch, C., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). Perceived control of life regrets: Good for young and bad for old adults. Psychology and Aging, 17, 340–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrosch, C., Heckhausen, J., & Lachman, M. E. (2000). Primary and secondary control strategies for managing health and financial stress across adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 15, 387–399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrosch, C., Miller, G. E., Scheier, M. F., & Pontet, C. (2007). Giving up on unattainable goals: Benefits for health?Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 251–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., Miller, G. E., Schulz, R., & Carver, C. S. (2003). Adaptive self-regulation of unattainable goals: Goal disengagement, goal reengagement and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29 (12), 1494–1508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrosch, C., Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). Health stresses and depressive symptomatology in the elderly: The importance of health engagement control strategies. Health Psychology, 21 (4), 340–348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrosch, C., Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (2004). Health stresses and depressive symptomatology in the elderly: A control-process approach. Current Directions, 13, 17–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wundt, W. (1874). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie [Principles of physiological psychology]. Leipzig, Germany: Engelmann.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1894). Über psychische Causalität und das Princip des psychophysischen Parallelismus [On psychic causality and the principle of psychopsychic parallelism]. Philosophische Studien, 10, 1–124.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1896). Grundriß der Psychologie [Outlines of psychology]. Leipzig, Germany: Engelmann.Google Scholar
Wynne-Edwards, K. E. (2001). Hormonal changes in mammalian fathers. Hormones and Behavior, 40, 139–145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamaguchi, S., & Ninomiya, K. (2000). Umami and food palatability. Journal of Nutrition, 130 (4 Suppl), 921–926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yarrow, L. J., McQuiston, S., MacTurk, R. H., McCarthy, M. E., Klein, R. P., & Vietze, P. M. (1983). Assessment of mastery motivation during the first year of life: Contemporaneous and cross-age relationships. Developmental Psychology, 19, 159–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative and Neurological Psychology, 18, 459–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yerkes, R. M., & Morgulis, S. (1909). The method of Pavlov in animal psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 6, 257–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, L. J., & Insel, T. R. (2002). Hormones and parental behavior. In Becker, J. B., Breedlove, S. M., Crews, D., & McCarthy, M. M. (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinology (2nd ed., pp. 331–369). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Young, P. T. (1936). Motivation of behavior: The fundamental determinants of human and animal activity. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, P. T. (1941). The experimental analysis of appetite. Psychological Bulletin, 38, 129–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, P. T. (1949). Food-seeking drive, affective process, and learning. Psychological Review, 56, 98–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, P. T. (1959). The role of affective processes in learning and motivation. Psychological Review, 66, 104–125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, P. T. (1961). Motivation and emotion: A survey of the determinants of human and animal activity. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yussen, S., & Kane, P. (1985). Children's conceptions of intelligence. In Yussen, S. R. (Ed.), The growth of reflection in children (pp. 207–241). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Cognitive theories in social psychology. In Lindzey, G. & Aronson, E. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. I, 2nd ed., pp. 320–411). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zak, P. J., Kurzban, R., & Matzner, W. T. (2005). Oxytocin is associated with human trustworthiness. Hormones and Behavior, 48 (5), 522–527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeaman, D. (1949). Response latency as a function of the amount of reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 466–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zehr, J. L., Maestripieri, D., & Wallen, K. (1998). Estradiol increases female sexual initiation independent of male responsiveness in rhesus monkeys. Hormones and Behavior, 33, 95–103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Über das Behalten von erledigten und unerledigten Handlungen [The retention of completed and uncompleted activities]. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1–85.Google Scholar
Zepelin, H., Sills, R. A., & Heath, M. W. (1986–87). Is age becoming irrelevant? An exploratory study of perceived age norms. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 241–256.Google Scholar
Ziegler, A., & Heller, K. A. (2000). Effects of an attribution retraining with female students gifted in physics. Journal of the Education of the Gifted, 23, 217–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, A., & Stöger, H. (2004). Evaluation of an attributional retraining (modeling technique) to reduce gender differences in chemistry instruction. High Ability Studies, 15, 63–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimbardo, P. (ed.) (1969). The cognitive control of motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (1997). Developmental phases in self-regulation: Shifting from process goals to outcome goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 29–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, M., & Lubin, B. (1965). Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist: Today form. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.Google Scholar
Zumkley, H. (1978). Aggression und Katharsis [Aggression and catharsis]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Zurbriggen, E. L. (2000). Social motives and cognitive power-sex associations: Predictors of aggressive sexual behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 559–581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A. P., & Midden, C. (1999). To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviors. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 971–979.3.0.CO;2-A>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abel, T. M. (1938). Neuro-circularity reaction and the recall of unfinished and completed tasks. Journal of Psychology, 6, 377–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abele, A. E., Andrä, M. S., & Schute, T. (1999). Wer hat nach dem Hochschulexamen schnell eine Stelle? Erste Ergebnisse der Erlanger Längsschnittstudie (BELA-E) [Who enters a job quickly after graduating? Results of a longitudinal study (BELA-E)]. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 43, 95–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96, 358–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ach, N. (1905). Über die Willenstätigkeit und das Denken [On the activity of will and on thinking]. Göttingen, Germany: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.Google Scholar
Ach, N. (1910). Über den Willensakt und das Temperament [On acts of will and temperament]. Leipzig, Germany: Quelle & Meyer.Google Scholar
Ach, N. (1935). Analyse des Willens [Analysis of the will]. In Abderhalden, E. (Ed.), Handbuch der biologischen Arbeitsmethoden (Vol. 6, Part E 460). Berlin, Germany: Urban & Schwarzberg.Google Scholar
Achtziger, A. (2003). Kognitionspsychologische Aspekte der willentlichen Stereotypkontrolle [Cognitive aspects of voluntary stereotype control]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Konstanz, Germany.Google Scholar
Achtziger, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). Controling stereotypes and prejudice by implementation intentions. Paper presented at the Small Group Meeting of the EASP, Jena, Germany.Google Scholar
Achtziger, A., Rockstroh, B., Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2003, September). Neurophysiologische Korrelate verschiedener handlungsvorbereitender mentaler Zustände [Neurophysiological correlates of goal setting strategies]. Paper presented at the 9th meeting of the Fachgruppe Sozialpsychologie, Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Achtziger, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (in press). Implementation intentions and shielding goal striving from unwanted thoughts and feelings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.Google Scholar
Achtziger, A., Michalski, V., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (forthcoming). Processing stereotype-inconsistent information under cognitive load.
Achtziger, A., Moratti, S., Jaudas, A., Rockstroh, B., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (forthcoming). The control of automatic stereotype activation by means of implementation intentions in the EEG.
Adams, R. E., & Passman, R. H. (1979). Effects of visual and auditory aspects of mothers and stranger on the play and exploration of children. Developmental Psychology, 15(3), 269–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adler, A. (1922). Über den nervösen Charakter [The neurotic constitution] (3rd ed.). Munich, Germany: Bergmann.Google Scholar
Adler, D. L., & Kounin, J. S. (1939). Some factors operating at the moment of resumption of interrupted tasks. Journal of Psychology, 7, 255–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adolphs, R., & Tranel, D. (2000). Emotion recognition and the human amygdala. In Aggleton, J. P. (Ed.), The amygdala: A functional analysis (pp. 587–630). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Aellig, S. (2004). Über den Sinn des Unsinns. Flow-Erleben und Wohlbefinden als Anreize für autotelische Tätigkeiten [The sense in not being sensible: Flow experience and well-being as motivation for autotelic activities]. Münster, Germany: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., Connor, O' E., & Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32(3), 537–551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Affleck, G., Tennen, H., Croog, S., & Levine, S. (1987). Causal attribution, perceived benefits and morbidity after a heart attack: An 8 year study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 29–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainley, M. D. (1993). Styles of engagement with learning: Multidimensional assessment of their relationship with strategy use and school achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), 395–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. (1972). Attachment and dependency: A comparison. In Gewirtz, J. (Ed.), Attachment and dependency (pp. 251). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. (1979). Infant-mother attachment. American Psychologist, 34, 932–937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ainsworth, M. D., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration and separation: Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child Development, 41(1), 49–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. (1974). Infant mother attachment and social development: Socialisation as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In Richards, R. P. M. (Ed.), The integration of a child into a social world. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. N. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1969). The prediction of behavioral intentions in a choice situation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 5, 400–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alberch, P. (1980). Ontogenesis and morphological diversification. American Zoologist, 20, 653–667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albert, D. J., Jonik, R. H., & Walsh, M. L. (1992). Hormone-dependent aggression in male and female rats: Experiential, hormonal, and neural foundations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 16(2), 177–192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albert, D. J., Petrovic, D. M., Walsh, M. L., & Jonik, R. H. (1989). Medial accumbens lesions attenuate testosterone-dependent aggression in male rats. Physiology & Behavior, 46, 625–631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alker, H. A. (1972). Is personality situationally specific or intrapsychically consistent?Journal of Personality, 40, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allensbacher Markt- and Werbeträgeranalyse (AWA). (1995–2000) (2000). Berichtsband I. Markstrukturen [Technical reports I. Market structures]. Allensbach, Germany: Institut für Demoskopie.
Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser?Journal of Experimental Psychology, 108, 441–485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1988). Depressive realism: Four theoretical perspectives. In Alloy, L. B. (Ed.), Cognitive processes in depression (pp. 223–265). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1953). The trend in motivation theory. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 23, 107–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, G. W., & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait-names: A psycholexical study. Psychological Monographs, 47(211).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alper, T. G. (1946). Task-orientation vs. ego-orientation in learning and retention. American Journal of Psychology, 59, 236–248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alper, T. G. (1957). Predicting the direction of selective recall: Its relation to ego strength and n achievement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55, 149–165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altshuler, J. A., & Ruble, D. N. (1989). Developmental changes in children's awareness of strategies for coping with uncontrollable stress. Child Development, 60, 1337–1349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ames, C. (1978). Children's achievement attributions and self reinforcement: Effects of self concept and competitive reward structure. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 345–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ames, C., & Ames, R. (1984). Systems of student and teacher motivation: Toward a qualitative definition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 535–556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ames, C., Ames, R., & Felker, D. W. (1977). Effects of competitive reward structure and valence of outcome on children's achievement attributions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: Students' learning strategies and motivational processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(3), 260–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, C., & Berdahl, J. L. (2002). Examining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1362–1377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, C. A. (1983). Imagination and expectation: The effect of imagining behavioral scripts on personal intentions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 293–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition: Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Andersen, S. M., & Chen, S. U. (2002). The relational self: An interpersonal social-cognitive theory. Psychological Review, 109, 619–645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andresen, B. (1995). Risikobereitschaft (R): Der sechste Basisfaktor der Persönlichkeit: Konvergenz multivariater Studien und Konstruktexplikation [“Risk preference” (R) – The sixth basic factor of personality: Convergence of multivariate studies and construct explication]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 16, 210–236.Google Scholar
Andrews, J. D. W. (1967). The achievement motive and advancement in two types of organizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 163–168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angermeier, W. F., & Peters, M. (1973). Bedingte Reaktionen [Conditioned reactions]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angleitner, A., & Ostendorf, F. (1994). Temperament and the Big Five factors of personality. In Halverson, C. F., Kohnstamm, G. A., & Martin, R. P. (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 69–90). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (2003). The effects of mindset on behavior: Self-regulation in deliberative and implemental frames of mind. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 86–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, R. M. (1960). Emotion and personality: Vol. I: Psychological aspects, Vol. II: Neurological and psychological aspects. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1962). Performance expectancy as a determinant of actual performance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 178–183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asendorpf, J. B. (1984). Shyness, embarrassment, and self-presentation: A control theory approach. In Schwarzer, R. (Ed.), The self in anxiety, stress, and depression (pp. 109–114). Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
Asendorpf, J. B. (2004). Psychologie der Persönlichkeit [Psychology of personality]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asendorpf, J. B., Banse, R., & Mücke, D. (2002). Double dissociation between implicit and explicit personality self-concept: The case of shy behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 380–393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asendorpf, J. B., Weber, A., & Burkhardt, K. (1994). Zur Mehrdeutigkeit projektiver Testergebnisse: Motiv-Projektionen oder Thema-Sensitivität? [On the multiple meaning of projective tests: Motive projection or thematic sensitivity?]Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 15, 155–165.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1953). The achievement motive and recall of interrupted and completed tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 381–390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359–372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.). (1958a). Motives in fantasy, action, and society. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1958b). Towards experimental analysis of human motivation in terms of motives, expectancies and incentives. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 288–305). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1964a). An introduction to motivation. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1964b). Some neglected variables in contemporary conceptions of decision and performance. Psychological Reports, 14, 575–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1974a). Motivational determinants of intellective performance and cumulative achievement. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 389–410). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1974b). Strength of motivation and efficiency of performance. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 193–218). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1981). Studying personality in the context of an advanced motivational psychology. American Psychologist, 36, 171–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1987). Michigan studies of fear of failure. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 47–59). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. (1970). The dynamics of action. New York: Wiley.Google ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. A. (1978). Introduction to motivation (2nd ed.). New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., Bongort, K., & Price, L. H. (1977). Explorations using computer simulation to comprehend TAT measurement of motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., & Cartwright, D. (1964). Some neglected variables in contemporary conceptions of decision and performance. Psychological Reports, 14, 575–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., & Feather, N. T. (Eds.). (1966). A theory of achievement motivation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., Heyns, R. W., & Veroff, J. (1954). The effect of experimental arousal of the affiliation motive on thematic apperception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 405–410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. W., Lens, W., & O'Malley, P. M. (1976). Motivation and ability: Interactive psychological determinants of intellective performance, educational achievement, and each other. In Sewell, W. H., Hauser, R. M., & Featherman, D. L. (Eds.), Schooling and achievement in American society (pp. 29–60). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., & Litwin, G. H. (1960). Achievement motive and test anxiety conceived as motive to approach success and motive to avoid failure. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 52–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. W., & McClelland, D. C. (1948). The projective expression of needs: II. The effect of different intensities of the hunger drive in thematic apperception. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33, 643–658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., & Raynor, J. O. (1974). Motivation and achievement. Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., & Reitman, W. R. (1956). Performance as a function of motive strength and expectancy of goal attainment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53, 361–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. W., & Walker, E. L. (1956). The affiliation motive and perceptual sensitivity to faces. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53, 38–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and content. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 338–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aveling, F. (1926). The psychology of conation and will. British Journal of Psychology, 16, 339–353.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 5–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Boston: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Balagura, S. (1973). Hunger: A biopsychological analysis. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (1998). Life-span theory in developmental psychology. In Damon, W. & Lerner, R. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 1029–1143). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: A metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. American Psychologist, 55, 122–135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Band, E. B., & Weisz, J. R. (1988). How to feel better when it feels bad: Children's perspectives on coping with everyday stress. Developmental Psychology, 24, 246–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1971). Vicarious and self-reinforcement processes. In Glaser, R. (Ed.), The nature of reinforcement (pp. 228–278). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism. American Psychologist, 33, 344–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanisms in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1991). Self-regulation of motivation through anticipatory and self-reactive mechanisms. In Dienstbier, R. A. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 38. Perspectives on motivation (pp. 69–164). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1998). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Banse, R. (2001). Affective priming with liked and disliked persons: Prime visibility determines congruency and incongruency effects. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 501–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bär, R. (1998). Pornographie: Motive, Meinungen, sexuelle Phantasien und situative Kontexte [Pornography: Motives, opinions, sexual fantasies, and situational contexts]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Wuppertal, Germany.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of social interaction. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 93–130). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In Wyer, R. S. & Srull, T. (Eds), Handbook of social cognition: Vol. 1. Basic processes (2nd ed., pp. 1–41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In Wyer, R. S. (Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Barndollar, K. (1996). Automaticity in action: The unconscious as repository of chronic goals and motivation. In Gollwitzer, P. M. & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 457–481). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes, C. (1996). The automatic evaluation effect: Unconditional automatic attitude activation with a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 104–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype priming on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. L. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: On the automaticity of higher mental processes. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 925–945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bargh, J. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1994). Environmental control of goal-directed action: Automatic and strategic contingencies between situations and behavior. In Spaulding, W. D. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 41. Integrative views of motivation, cognition, and emotion (pp. 71–124). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Barker, R. G., & Gump, P. (Eds.). (1964). Big school, small school: High school size and student behavior. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Barnett, P. A., & Gotlib, I. C. (1988). Psychological functioning and depression: Distinguishing among antecedents, concomitants, and consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 97–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, K. C., & Morgan, G. A. (1995). Continuities and discontinuities in mastery motivation during infancy and toddlerhood: A conceptualization and review. In MacTurk, R. H. & Morgan, G. A. (Eds.), Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptualization and applications (pp. 57–93). Westport, CT: Ablex.Google Scholar
Barrett, K. C., Morgan, G. A., & Maslin-Cole, B. (1993). Three studies on the development of mastery motivation in infancy and toddlerhood. In Messer, D. (Ed.), Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and social processes (pp. 83–108). London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barron, K. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement goals and optimal motivation: Testing multiple goal models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 706–722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love. NeuroReport, 11(17), 3829–3834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. NeuroImage, 21(3), 1155–1166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartlett, E. W., & Smith, C. P. (1966). Childrearing practices, birth order and the development of achievement-related motives. Psychological Reports, 19, 1207–1216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baum, M. J. (1992). Neuroendocrinology of sexual behavior in the male. In Becker, J. B., Breedlove, S. M., & Crews, D. (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinology (pp. 97–130). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baumann, N., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Affect regulation and motive-incongruent achievement orientation: Antecedents of subjective well-being and symptom formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 781–799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2002). Intuition, affect and personality: Unconscious coherence judgments and self-regulation of negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1213–1223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Self-infiltration: Confusing assigned tasks as self-selected in memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 487–497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2004). How to resist temptation: The effects of external control versus autonomy support on the dynamics of self-regulation. Journal of Personality, 73, 443–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumann, N., Kuhl, J., & Kazén, (2004). Hemispheric activation and self infiltration: Testing a neuropsychological model of internalization. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 135–163.
Baumeister, R. F. (1984). Choking under pressure: Self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 610–620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Ego-depletion and the self's executive function. In Tesser, A. & Felson, R. B. (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on self and identity (pp. 9–33). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., & Showers, C. (1986). A review of paradoxical performance effects: Choking under pressure in sports and mental tests. European Journal of Social Psychology, 16, 361–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayer, U. C., Achtziger, A., Malzacher, J. T., Moskowitz, G. B., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (under review). Strategic automaticity by implementation intentions: Action initiation without conscious intent.
Baxter, M. G., & Murray, E. A. (2002). The amygdala and reward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(7), 563–573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayer, U. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). Mindset effects on information search in self-evaluation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 313–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayer, U. C., Jaudas, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2002, July). Do implementation intentions facilitate switching between tasks? Poster presented at the International Symposium on Executive Functions, Konstanz, Germany.Google Scholar
Beauregard, M., Levesque, J., & Bourgouin, P. (2001). Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 6993–7000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 295–307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275(5304), 1293–1295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression – Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Beckmann, J. (1984). Kognitive Dissonanz: Eine handlungstheoretische Perspektive [Cognitive dissonance: An action theory perspective]. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckmann, J. (1994). Rumination and deactivation of an intention. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 317–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckmann, J. (1996). Self-presentation and the Zeigarnik effect. In Gjesme, T. & Nygard, R. (Ed.), Advances in motivation (pp. 35–45). Oslo, Norway: Scandinavian University Press.Google Scholar
Beckmann, J., Bobka, K., Fehrenbach, H., Hellebrandt, M., & Rost, K. (2007). The perseverance of complete and incomplete intentions in memory: Zeigarnik effect or self serving recall?Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Beckmann, J., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Deliberative versus implemental states of mind: The issue of impartiality in predecisional and postdecisional information processing. Social Cognition, 5, 259–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckmann, J., & Kuhl, J. (1984). Altering information to gain action control: Functional aspects of human information processing in decision making. Journal of Research in Personality, 18, 224–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckmann, J., & Rolstad, K. (1997). Aktivierung, Selbstregulation und Leistung: Gibt es so etwas wie Übermotivation? [Activation, self-regulation, and performance: Is there such a thing as overmotivation?]Sportwissenschaft, 27, 23–37.Google Scholar
Beeman, M., Friedman, R. B., Grafman, J., Perez, E., Diamond, S., & Lindsay, M. B. (1994). Summation priming and coarse coding in the right hemisphere. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 26–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beit-Hallahmi, B. (1980). Achievement motivation and economic growth: A replication. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 210–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bem, D. J., & Allen, A. (1974). On predicting some of the people some of the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies in behavior. Psychological Review, 81, 506–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beneson, J., & Dweck, C. S. (1986). The development of trait explanation and self-evaluations in the academic and social domains. Child Development, 57, 1179–1189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, P., & Luckman, T. (1967). The social construction of reality. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Berkowitz, L. (1974). Some determinants of impulsive aggression: Role of mediated associations with reinforcements for aggression. Psychological Review, 81, 165–176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkowitz, L. (1990). On the formation and regulation of anger and aggression: A cognitive-neoassociationistic analysis. American Psychologist, 45, 494–503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. New York: MacGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Berkowitz, L. (1994). Is something missing? Some observations prompted by the cognitive-neoassociationist view of anger and emotional aggression. In Huesmann, L. R. (Ed.), Aggressive behaviour: Current perspectives (pp. 35–57). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Berkowitz, L., & LePage, A. (1967). Weapons as aggression-eliciting stimuli. Journal of Personalitiy and Social Psychology, 7, 202–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1963a). Complexity and incongruity variables as determinants of exploratory choice and evaluative ratings. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 17, 274–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1963b). Motivational problems raised by exploratory and epistemic behavior. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. V, pp. 284–364). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1965). Structure and direction in thinking. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1967). Arousal and reinforcement. In Levine, D. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1967 (pp. 1–110). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1973). The vicissitudes of aplopathematic and thelematoscopic pneumatology (or the hydrography of hedonism). In Berlyne, D. E. & Madsen, K. B. (Eds.), Pleasure, reward, preference (pp. 1–34). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (Ed.). (1974). Studies in the new experimental aesthetics. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E., & Crozier, J. B. (1971). Effects of complexity and prechoice stimulation on exploratory choice. Perception and Psychophysics, 10, 242–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernhardt, P. C., Dabbs, J. M., Jr., Fielden, J. A., & Lutter, C. D. (1998). Testosterone changes during vicarious experiences of winning and losing among fans at sporting events. Physiology & Behavior, 65, 59–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernoulli, D. (1738). Specimen theoriae novae de mensura sortis. Commentarii Academiae Scientiarum Imperialis Petropolitanae, 5, 175–192.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (1996). Food reward: Brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 20, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (2003). Comparing the emotional brains of humans and other animals. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 25–51). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience?Brain Research Review, 28, 309–369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (2003). Parsing reward. Trends in Neurosciences, 26(9), 507–513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beswick, G., & Mann, L. (1994). State orientation and procrastination. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Betancourt, H., & Blair, I. (1992). An attribution-emotion model of violence in conflict situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 343–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bexton, W. H., Heron, W., & Scott, T. H. (1954). Effects of decreased variation in the sensory environment. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 8, 70–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bieneck, A. (1991). Tätigkeitszentrierte Anreize des Skifahrens für Behinderte und Nichtbehinderte in Abhängigkeit vom Fähigkeitsstand [Activity-related incentives of skiing for disabled and nondisabled individuals as a function of ability level]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Bieri, P. (2001). Das Handwerk der Freiheit: Die Entdeckung des eigenen Willens [The craft of free time: Discovering one's own will]. Munich, Germany: Hanser.Google Scholar
Biernat, M. (1989). Motives and values to achieve: Different constructs with different effects. Journal of Personality, 57, 69–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billington, C. J., & Levine, A. S. (1992). Hypothalamic neuropeptide regulation of feeding and energy metabolism. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 2(6), 847–851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bindra, D. (1959). Motivation: A systematic reinterpretation.New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
Bindra, D. (1969). The interrelated mechanisms of reinforcement and motivation, and the nature of their influence on response. In Arnold, W. J. & Levine, D. (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1969 (pp. 1–38). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Bindra, D. (1974). A motivational view of learning, performance, and behavior modification. Psychological Review, 81, 199–213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bindra, D. (1978). How adaptive behavior is produced: A perceptual-motivational alternative to response-reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 41–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birenbaum, G. (1930). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs- und Affektpsychologie. VII. Das Vergessen einer Vornahme [Studies on the psychology of action and emotion. VIII. The forgetting of instructions]. Psychologische Forschung, 13, 1930, 218–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birney, R. C., Burdick, H., & Teevan, R. C. (1969). Fear of failure motivation.New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Bischof, N. (1975). A systems approach towards the functional connections of attachment and fear. Child Development, 46, 801–817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bischof, N. (1985). Das Rätsel Ödipus: Die biologischen Wurzeln des Urkonfliktes von Intimität und Autonomie [The enigma of Oedipus: Biological roots of the basic conflict between intimacy and autonomy]. Munich, Germany: Piper.Google Scholar
Bischof, N. (1993). Untersuchungen zur Systemanalyse der sozialen Motivation I: Die Regulation der sozialen Distanz – Von der Feldtheorie zur Systemtheorie [Social distance regulation – From “field” to “system”]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 201, 5–43.Google Scholar
Bischof, N. (1996). Untersuchungen zur Systemanalyse der sozialen Motivation IV: Die Spielarten des Lächelns und das Problem der motivationalen Sollwertanpassung [The varieties of smiling and the problem of motivational acclimatization]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 204, 1–40.Google Scholar
Bischoff, J. (2003). Lernmotivation, Flow-Erleben und Leistung in universitären Fremdsprachkursen [Motivation to learn, flow experience, and achievement in university second language courses]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Potsdam, Germany.Google Scholar
Bischof-Köhler, D. (1985). Zur Phylogenese menschlicher Motivation [On the phylogeny of human motivation]. In Eckensberger, L. H. & Lantermann, E.-D. (Eds.), Emotion und Reflexivität (pp. 3–47). Munich, Germany: Urban & Schwarzenberg.Google Scholar
Bitterman, M. E. (1975). The comparative analysis of learning. Science, 188, 699–709.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Björkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., & Kaukiainen, A. (1992). Do girls manipulate and boys fight? Developmental trends in regard to direct and indirect aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 18, 117–127.3.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Björkqvist, K., & Niemelä, P. (Eds.). (1992). Of mice and women: Aspects of female aggression. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Blankenship, V. (1984). Computer anxiety and self-concept of ability. In Schwarzer, R. (Ed.), The self in anxiety, stress, and depression (pp. 151–158). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Blankenship, V. (1987). A computer-based measure of resultant achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 361–372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the organization of behavior. In Collins, W. A. (Ed.), Development of cognition, affect, and social relations: The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 39–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Blodgett, H. C. (1929). The effect of the introduction of reward upon the maze performance of rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 113–134.Google Scholar
Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. PNAS, 98(20), 11818–11823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blossfeld, H. P. (1987). Labor market entry and the sexual segregation of careers in the Federal Republic of Germany. American Journal of Sociology, 93, 89–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blossfeld, H. P. (1988). Sensible Phasen im Bildungsverlauf – Eine Längsschnittanalyse über die Prägung von Bildungskarrieren durch den gesellschaftlichen Wandel [Critical phases in educational careers – A longitudinal study on the influence of societal changes on educational careers]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 34, 45–63.Google Scholar
Blossfeld, H. P., & Mayer, K. U. (1988). Labor market segmentation in the Federal Republic of Germany: An empirical study of segmentation theories from a life course perspective. European Sociological Review, 4, 123–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bludau, H. F. (1976). Mobilitätstendenzen im Gefangenen-Dilemma-Spiel [Mobility tendencies in the prisoner's dilemma game]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Blunt, A., & Pychyl, T. A. (1998). Volitional action and inaction in the lives of undergraduate students: State orientation, procrastination and proneness to boredom. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 837–846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boekaerts, M. (2003). Towards a model that integrates motivation, affect and learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, Monograph Series II, Part 2 (Development and Motivation: Joint Perspectives), 173–189.Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1965). Readiness to eat: Effects of age, sex and weight loss. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 60, 88–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolles, R. C. (1967). Theory of motivation.New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1972). Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning. Psychological Review, 79, 394–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1974). Cognition and motivation: Some historical trends. In Weiner, B. (Ed.), Cognitive views of human motivation (pp. 1–20). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1975). Theory of motivation (2nd ed.). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Bolte, A., Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Emotion and intuition: Effects of positive and negative mood on implicit judgments of semantic coherence. Psychological Science, 14, 416–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonsfield, W. A. (1953). The occurrence of clustering in the recall of randomly arranged associates. Journal of General Psychology, 49, 229–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1929). A history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, F. (1993). NEO-Fünf-Faktoren-Inventar (NEO-FFI) nach Costa und McCrae: Handanweisung [The Neo Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) by Costa and McCrae: Manual]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Bornstein, R. F. (2002). A process dissociation approach to objective-projective test score interrelationships. Journal of Personality Assessment, 78, 47–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosson, J. K., Swann, W. B., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant revisited?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 631–643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouton, M. E., & Fanselow, M. S. (1997). Learning, motivation and cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Bower, G. H. (1981). Emotional mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowers, K. S. (1973). Situationism in psychology: An analysis and a critique. Psychological Review, 80, 307–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowi, U. (1990). Der Einfluss von Motiven auf Zielsetzung und Zielrealisation [The influence of motives on goal setting and goal implementation]. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child's tie to his mother. Interactional Journal of Psychoanalysis, 39, 350–373.Google ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. In Khan, M. M. (Ed.), Attachment 1. London: Hogarth.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd rev. ed.). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boyatzis, R. E. (1972). A two factor theory of affiliation motivation.. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Boyatzis, R. E. (1973). Affiliation motivation. In McClelland, D. C. & Steele, R. S. (Eds.), Human motivation: A book of readings (pp. 253–276). Morristown, NJ: General Learning.Google Scholar
Boyce, R. (1976). In the shadow of Darwin. In Green, R. G. & Neil, E. C. O' (Eds.), Perspectives in aggression (pp. 11–35). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Brackhane, R. (1976). Bezugssysteme im Leistungsverhalten [Reference systems in achievement behavior]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Münster, Germany.Google Scholar
Bradley, G. W. (1978). Self-serving biases in the attribution process: A reexamination of the fact or fiction question. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 56–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandtstädter, J. (1984). Personal and social control over development: Some implications of an action perspective in life-span developmental psychology. In Baltes, P. B. & Brim, O. G. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 6) (pp. 1–32). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Brandtstädter, J. (1986). Personale Entwicklungskontrolle und entwicklungsregulatives Handeln: Überlegungen und Befunde zu einem vernachlässigten Forschungsthema [Personal control and regulative action in development: Thoughts and findings on an underrated issue of research]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 18, 316–334.Google Scholar
Brandtstädter, J. (1998). Action perspectives on human development. Handbook of Child Psychology, 1, 807–863.Google Scholar
Brandtstädter, J. (2001). Entwicklung, Intentionalität, Handeln [Development, intentionality, action]. Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Brandtstädter, J., & Greve, W. (1994). The aging self: Stabilizing and protective processes. Developmental Review, 14, 52–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandtstädter, J., & Lerner, R. (Eds.). (1999). Action and self development: Theory and research through the life span. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Brandtstädter, J., & Renner, G. (1990). Tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal adjustment: Explication and age-related analysis of assimilative and accommodative strategies of coping. Psychology and Aging 5, 58–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandtstädter, J., & Rothermund, K. (2002). The life-course dynamics of goal pursuit and goal adjustment: A two process framework. Developmental Review, 22, 117–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandtstädter, J., & Rothermund, K. (2003). Intentionality and time in human development and aging: Compensation and goal adjustment in changing developmental contexts. In Staudinger, U. M. & Lindenberger, U. (Eds.), Understanding human development: Dialogues with lifespan psychology (pp. 105–124). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandtstädter, J., Wentura, D., & Rothermund, K. (1999). Intentional self-development through adulthood and later life: Tenacious pursuit and flexible adjustment of goals. In Brandstädter, J. & Lerner, R. (Eds.), Action and self development: Theory and research through the life span (pp. 373–400). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Brandstätter, V., & Frank, E. (2002). Effects of deliberative and implemental mindsets on persistence in goal-directed behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1366–1378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946–960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brauckmann, L. (1976). Erstellung und Erprobung eines Lehrerverhaltenstrainings zur Veränderung der motivanregenden Bedingungen des Unterrichts[Development and testing of a teacher training program targeting the motivating conditions of instruction]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Braun, W., & Proebsting, H. (1986). Heiratstafeln verwitweter Deutscher 1979/82 und geschiedener Deutscher 1980/83 [Marriage tables of widowed, 1979/82, and divorced, 1980/83, Germans]. Wirtschaft und Statistik, 107–112.Google Scholar
Breckler, S. J., & Greenwald, A. G. (1986). Motivational facets of the self. In Sorrentino, R. M. & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (pp. 145–164). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Brehm, J. W. (1956). Post decision changes in the desirability of alternatives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 384–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehm, J. W., & Cohen, A. R. (1962). Explorations in cognitive dissonance. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breland, K., & Breland, M. (1961). The misbehavior of animals. American Psychologist, 16, 681–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In Srull, T. K. & Wyer, R. S. Jr. (Eds.), A dual process model of impression formation (pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brewin, C. R., Dalgleish, T., & Joseph, S. (1996). A dual representation theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Review, 103, 670–686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brock, D. B., Guralnick, J. M., & Brody, J. A. (1990). Demography and epidemiology of aging in the United States: Handbook of the biology of aging (Vol. 3, pp. 3–23). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1988). The ecology of developmental processes. In Damon, W. & Lerner, M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 993–1028). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brown, J. F. (1933). Über die dynamischen Eigenschaften der Realitäts- und Irrealitätsschichten [On the dynamic properties of the levels of reality and unreality]. Psychologische Forschung, 18, 1–26.Google Scholar
Brown, J. S. (1948). Gradients of approach and avoidance responses and their relation to model of motivation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 41, 450–465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J. S. (1953). Problems presented by the concept of acquired drives. In Brown, J. S. & Jacobs, A. (Eds.), Current theory and research in motivation: A symposium (pp. 1–21). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Brown, J. S. (1961). The motivation of behavior.New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, P. L., & Jenkins, H. M. (1968). Auto-shaping of the pigeon's key-peck. Journal of Analysis of Behavior, 11, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunstein, J. C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1061–1070.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C. (1995). Motivation nach Misserfolg: Zur Bedeutung von Commitment und Substitution [Motivation following failure: The role of commitment and substitution]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Persönliche Ziele und subjektives Wohlbefinden bei älteren Menschen [Personal goals and subjective well-being among older adults]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 20, 58–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C. (2001). Persönliche Ziele und Handlungs- versus Lageorientierung: Wer bindet sich an realistische und bedürfniskongruente Ziele? [Personal goals and action versus state orientation: Who builds a commitment to realistic and need-congruent goals?]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 22, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C. (2003). Implizite Motive und motivationale Selbstbilder: Zwei Prädiktoren mit unterschiedlicher Gültigkeit [Implicit motives and motivational self-concepts: Two predictors with differing validity]. In Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. & Rheinberg, F. (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept (Tests and Trends Vol. 2, pp. 59–88). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., Ganserer, J., Maier, H. & Heckhausen, H. (1991). Persönliche Anliegen in Alltagssituationen [Personal concerns in everyday situations]. Memorandum No. 82.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1996). Effects of failure on subsequent performance: The importance of self-defining goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 395–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunstein, J. C., & Hoyer, S. (2002). Implizites versus explizites Leistungsstreben: Befunde zur Unabhängigkeit zweier Motivationssysteme [Implicit versus explicit achievement strivings: Empirical evidence of the independence of two motivational systems]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 16, 51–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., Lautenschlager, U., Nawroth, B., Pöhlmann, K., & Schultheiss, O. C. (1995). Persönliche Anliegen, soziale Motive und emotionales Wohlbefinden [Personal goals, social motives, and emotional well-being]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 16, 1–10.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (1996). Persönliche Ziele: Ein Überblick zum Stand der Forschung [Personal goals: A state-of-the-art review]. Psychologische Rundschau, 47, 146–160.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (2001). Das Streben nach persönlichen Zielen: Emotionales Wohlbefinden und proaktive Entwicklung über die Lebensspanne [Striving for personal goals: Emotional well-being and proactive development across the lifespan]. In Jüttemann, G. & Thomae, H. (Eds.), Persönlichkeit und Entwicklung (pp. 155–188). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (2005). Implicit and self-attributed motives to achieve: Two separate but interacting needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 205–222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunstein, J. C., Maier, G. W., & Schultheiss, O. C. (1999). Motivation und Persönlichkeit: Von der Analyse von Teilsystemen zur Analyse ihrer Interaktionen [Motivation and personality: From the analysis of subsystems to the analyses of their interactions]. In Jerusalem, M. & Pekrun, R. (Eds.), Emotion, Motivation und Leistung (pp. 147–167). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Olbrich, E. (1985). Personal helplessness and action control: An analysis of achievement-related cognitions, self-assessments, and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1540–1551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Schmitt, C. H. (2003). Prüfung der konvergenten, diskriminanten und prädiktiven Validität von Leistungsmotiv-IATs, -TATs und -Fragebögen [Testing the convergent, discriminant, and predictive validity of the achievement motive IAT, TAT, and questionnaires] (DFG Report). Potsdam, Germany: University of Potsdam.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Schmitt, C. H. (2004). Assessing individual differences in achievement motivation with the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 536–555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 494–508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Maier, G. W. (1999). The pursuit of personal goals: A motivational approach to well-being and life adjustment. In Brandtstädter, J. & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Action and self-development: Theory and research through the life span (pp. 169–196). London: Sage.Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1952). The conceptual frame work of psychology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and representative design of psychological experiments. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Bryk, A., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models for social and behavioral research: Applications and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Buber, M. (1979). I and thou. New York: Scribner's.Google Scholar
Buck, R. (1985). Prime theory: An integrated view of motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 389–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bühler, C. (1933). Der menschliche Lebenslauf als psychologisches Problem [The human life course as a topic of psychology]. Leipzig, Germany: Hirzel.Google Scholar
Bühler, C., Hetzer, H., & Mabel, F. (1928). Die Affektwirksamkeit von Fremdheitseindrücken im ersten Lebensjahr [The emotional effects of unfamiliarity in the first year of life]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 107, 30–40.Google Scholar
Bühler, C., & Marschak, M. (1969). Grundtendenzen des menschlichen Lebens [Basic tendencies of human life]. In Bühler, C. & Massarik, F. (Eds.), Lebenslauf und Lebensziele (pp. 78–88). Stuttgart, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Bühler, K. (1922). Die geistige Entwicklung des Kindes [The mental development of the child] (3rd ed.). Jena, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Bühler, K.-E., & Heim, G. (2002). Psychisches Trauma und fixe Ideen [Psychic trauma and fixed ideas]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 50, 394–408.Google Scholar
Bullock, M., & Lütkenhaus, P. (1988). The development of volitional behavior in the toddler years. Child Development, 59, 664–674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bullock, M., & Lütkenhaus, P. (1991). Who am I? Self understanding in toddlers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 217–238.Google Scholar
Buss, A. H. (1980). Self-consciousness and social anxiety. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1987). Selection, evocation and manipulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1214–1221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary personality psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 42, 459–491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, D. M. (2001). Human nature and culture: An evolutionary psychological perspective. Journal of Personality, 69, 955–978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204–232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, H. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Butler, R. (1993). Effects of task- and ego-achievement goals on information-seeking during task engagement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 18–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, R. (1999). Information seeking and achievement motivation in middle childhood and adolescence: The role of conceptions of ability. Developmental Psychology, 35, 146–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, R. (2000). What learners want to know: The role of achievement goals in shaping information seeking, learning, and interest. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 162–194). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Butterfield, E. C. (1964). The interruption of tasks: Methodological, factual and theoretical issues. Psychological Bulletin, 62, 309–322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butz, M. V., & Hoffmann, J. (2002). Anticipations control behavior: Animal behavior in an anticipatory learning classifier system. Adaptive Behavior, 10, 75–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, D. (1961a). Anxiety and the experimental arousal of affiliation need. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 660–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, D. (1961b). The repression-sensitization scale: Rationale, reliability, and validity. Journal of Personality, 29, 334–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, D. (1962). Response to attitude similarity-dissimilarity as a function of affiliation need. Journal of Personality, 30, 164–177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, D., McDonald, R. D., & Mikawa, J. (1963). Approach and avoidance affiliation motives. Journal of Personality, 31, 21–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabanac, M. (1971). Physiological role of pleasure. Science, 173(2), 1103–1107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cahill, L. (2000). Modulation of long-term memory in humans by emotional arousal: Adrenergic activation and the amygdala. In Aggleton, J. P. (Ed.), The amygdala: A functional analysis (pp. 425–446). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cahill, L., Prins, B., Weber, M., & McGaugh, J. L. (1994). Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature, 371, 702–704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calder, B., & Staw, B. M. (1975). The interaction of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Some methodological notes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 76–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, J., Banko, K. M., & Pierce, W. D. (2001). Pervasive negative effects of rewards on intrinsic motivation: The myth continues. The Behavior Analyst, 24, 1–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, B. A., & Sheffield, F. D. (1953). Relation of random activity to food deprivation. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 46, 320–322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, J. P., Dunnette, M. D., Lawler, E. E., & Weick, K. E. (1970). Managerial behavior performance and effectiveness. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Cannon, W. B., & Washburn, A. L. (1912). An explanation of hunger. American Journal of Physiology, 29, 441–454.Google Scholar
Canon, L. K. (1964). Self-confidence and selective exposure to information. In Festinger, L. (Ed.), Conflict, decision, and dissonance (pp. 83–95). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Cantor, N., & Fleeson, W. W. (1991). Life tasks and self-regulatory processes. In Maehr, M. L. & Pintrich, P. R. (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 7, pp. 327–269). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Cantor, N., Mischel, W., & Schwartz, J. C. (1982). A prototype analysis of psychological situations. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 45–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cantor, N., Norem, J. K., Niedenthal, M., & Brower, A. (1987). Life tasks, self-concept ideals, and cognitive strategies in a life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1178–1191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cantor, N., & Zirkel, S. (1990). Personality, cognition, and purposive behavior. In Pervin, L. A. (Ed.), Handbook of personality research: Theory and research (pp. 135–164). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cardinal, R. N., Parkinson, J. A., Hall, J., & Everitt, B. J. (2002). Emotion and motivation: The role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 321–352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, E. R. (1956). Attitude change through modification of attitude structure. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 256–261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, N. R. (1994). Physiology of behavior (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Caron, A. J., & Wallach, M. A. (1957). Recall of interrupted tasks under stress: A phenomenon of memory or learning?Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55, 372–381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caron, A. J., & Wallach, M. A. (1959). Personality determinants of repressive and obsessive reactions to failure stress. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 236–245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, S. J., Dabbs, J. M., & Carr, G. (1975). Mother-infant attachment: The importance of the mother's visual field. Child Development, 46(2), 331–338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, L., Voisey, J., & Daal, A. (2004). Mouse models of obesity. Clinics in Dermatology, 22(4), 345–349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 779–818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cartwright, D. (1942). The effect of interruption, completion and failure upon the attractiveness of activity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartwright, D. (1959). A field theory conception of power. In Cartwright, D. (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 183–220). Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Cartwright, D. (1965). Influence, leadership, and control. In March, J. G. (Ed.), Handbook of organizations (pp. 1–47). Chicago: Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Cartwright, D., & Festinger, L. (1943). A quantitative theory of decision. Psychological Review, 50, 595–621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S., Lawrence, J. W., & Scheier, M. E. (1996). A control-process perspective on the origins of affect. In Martin, L. L. & Tesser, A. (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 11–52). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A. (1998). Personality development across the life course. Handbook of Child Psychology, 3(5), 311–388.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1950). Personality: A systematic, theoretical, and factual study. New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1957). Personality and motiva tion: Structure and measurement. Yonkers, NY: World Book.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1958). Extracting the correct number of factors in factor analysis. Educational Psychological Measurement, 18, 791–838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1965). The scientific analysis of personality.Baltimore: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1974). Handbook of modern personality theory.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Cervone, D. (2004). The architecture of personality. Psychological Review, 111, 183–204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charlesworth, W. R. (1969). The role of surprise in cognitive development. In Elkind, D. & Flavell, J. H. (Eds.), Studies in cognitive development (pp. 257–314). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chasteen, A. L., Park, D., & Schwarz, N. (2001). Implementation intentions and facilitation of prospective memory. Psychological Science, 12, 457–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 330–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious behavioral confirmation processes: The self-fulfilling consequences of automatic stereotype activation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 541–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, P. W., & Novick, L. R. (1990a). Where is the bias in causal attribution? In Gilholey, K. J., Kayne, M. T. K., Logie, R. H., & Erdos, G. (Eds.), Lines of thinking (pp. 181–197). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cheng, P. W., & Novick, L. R. (1990b). A probabilistic contrast model of causal induction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 545–567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chipperfield, J. G., Pery, R. P., & Menec, A. (1999). Primary and secondary control-enhancing strategies: Implications for health in later life. Journal of Aging and Health, 11, 517–539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, K. (1999). Hypophysen-Gonaden-Achse (Mann) [Hypophyse-gonad axis (male)]. In Kirschbaum, C. & Hellhammer, D. (Eds.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Biologische Psychologie, Band 3: Psychoendokrinologie und Psychoimmunologie (pp. 141–222). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Chusmir, L. H. (1984). Motivational need pattern for police officers. Journal of Police Science & Administration, 12, 141–145.Google Scholar
Claessens, D. (1968). Instinkt, Psyche, Geltung: Bestimmungsfaktoren menschlichen Verhaltens. Eine soziologische Anthropologie [Instinct, psyche, status: Determining factors in human behavior. A sociological anthropology]. Köln (Cologne), Germany: Westdeutscher Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, P., & James, J. (1967). The effects of situation, attitude intensity and personality on information-seeking. Sociometry, 30, 235–245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, R. A. (1952). The projective measurement of experimental induced levels of sexual motivation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 391–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, R. A., & Sensibar, M. R. (1955). The relationship between symbolic and manifest projections of sexuality with some incidental correlates. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 50, 327–334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1973). Interactions between mothers and their young children: Characteristics and consequences. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(153), 6–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke-Stewart, K. A., VanderStoep, L. P., & Killian, P. (1979). Analysis and replication of mother-child relations at two years of age. Child Development, 50(3), 777–793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clavadetscher, C. (2003). Motivation ehrenamtlicher Arbeit im Verein Mahogany Hall, Bern [Motivation for voluntary work in the Mahogany Hall Society, Berne, Switzerland]. Unpublished final thesis, Berne University of Applied Sciences, Switzerland.Google Scholar
Clemente, C. D., & Chase, M. H. (1973). Neurological substrates of aggressive behavior. Annual Review of Physiology, 35, 329–356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coats, E. J., Janoff-Bulman, R., & Alpert, N. (1996). Approach versus avoidance goals: Differences in self-evaluation and well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1057–1067.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cofer, C. N., & Appley, M. H. (1964). Motivation: Theory and research. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Collins, C. J., Hanges, P. J., & Locke, E. A. (2004). The relation of achievement motivation to entrepreneurial behavior: A meta-analysis. Human Performance, 17, 95–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collis, G. M., & Schaffer, H. R. (1975). Synchronization of visual attention in mother-infant pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16, 315–320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compas, B. E. (1987). Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 393–403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compas, B. E., & Worsham, N. L. (1991, April). When mom or dad has cancer: Developmental differences in children's coping with family stress. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development meeting, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Compas, B. E., Worsham, N. L., Ey, L., & Howell, B. (1996). When mom or dad has cancer II: Coping, cognitive appraisals, and psychological distress in children of cancer patients. Health Psychology, 15, 167–175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Constantian, C. A. (1981). Attitudes, beliefs, and behavior in regard to spending time alone. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Parker, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261–288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, W. H. (1983). An achievement motivation nomological network. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 841–861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coopersmith, S. (1960). Self-esteem and need achievement as determinants of selective recall and repetition. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 310–317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corr, P. J., Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (1997). Personality, punishment, and procedural learning: A test of J. A. Gray's anxiety theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(2), 337–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 163–228). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1994). Origins of domain-specificity: The evolution of functional organization. In Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 85–116). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). From catalogue to classification: Murray's needs and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 258–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Covington, M. V. (1992). Making the grade: A self-worth perspective on motivation and school reform. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Covington, M. V., & Omelich, C. L. (1979). Are arousal attributions causal? A path analysis of the cognitive model of achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1487–1504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Covington, M. V., & Omelich, C. L. (1991). Need achievement revisited: Verification of Atkinson's original 2×2 model. In Spielberger, C. D., Sarason, I. G., Kulcsár, Z., & Heck, G. L. (Eds.), Stress and emotion: Anxiety, anger, and curiosity (Vol. 14, pp. 85–105). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Covington, M. V., & Roberts, B. W. (1994). Self-worth and college achievement: Motivational and personality correlates. In Pintrich, P. R., Brown, D. R., & Weinstein, C. E. (Eds.), Student motivation, cognition, and learning (pp. 157–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cowlishaw, G., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (1991). Dominance, rank, and mating success in male primates. Animal Behavior, 41, 1045–1056.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coyne, J. C., & Gotlib, I. C. H. (1983). The role of cognitions in depression: A critical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 472–505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, W. (1918). Appetites and aversions as constituents of instincts. Biological Bulletin of Woods Hole, 34, 91–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., Moroz, T. M., Moscovitch, M., Stuss, D. T., Winocur, G., Tulving, E., & Kapur, S. (1999). In search of the self: A positron emission tomography study. Psychological Science, 10, 26–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crespi, L. P. (1942). Quantitative variation of incentive and performance in the white rat. American Journal of Psychology, 55, 467–517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crespi, L. P. (1944). Amount of reinforcement and level of performance. Psychological Review, 51, 341–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cronbach, L. J. (1990). Essentials of psychological testing (5th ed.). New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Crozier, W. R. (1979). Shyness as anxious self-preoccupation. Psychological Reports, 44, 959–962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention. (1999). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. Gainesville: NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, University of Florida.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1991). Das Flow-Erlebnis und seine Bedeutung für die Psychologie des Menschen [The flow experience and its significance for human psychology]. In Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.), Die außergewöhnliche Erfahrung im Alltag. Die Psychologie des Flow-Erlebens (pp. 28–49). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997a). Dem Sinn des Lebens eine Zukunft geben [The evolving self: A psychology for the third millennium]. Stuttgart, Germany: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997b). Finding flow. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Cskiszentmihalyi, M., & Beattie, O. (1979). Life themes: A theoretical and empirical exploration of their origins and effects. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 19, 45–63.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R., & Prescott, S. (1977). The ecology of adolescence acivity and experience. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 6, 281–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 815–822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cube v., F. (2003). Lust auf Leistung [Appetite for achievement]. Munich, Germany: Piper.Google Scholar
Cunningham, J. D., & Kelley, H. H. (1975). Causal attributions for personal events of varying magnitudes. Journal of Personality, 43, 74–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dabbs, J. M., Frady, R. L., Carr, T. S., & Besch, N. F. (1987). Saliva testosterone and criminal violence in young adult prison inmates. Psychosomatic Medicine, 49, 174–182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dabbs, J. M., & Hargrove, M. F. (1997). Age, testosterone, and behavior among female prison inmates. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 477–480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, R. A. (1957). The concept of power. Behavioral Sciences, 2, 201–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1983). Sex, evolution, and behavior (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. London: Papermac.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (2000). Ich fühle also bin ich: Die Entschlüsselung des Bewusstseins [I feel, therefore I am: Decoding consciousness]. Munich, Germany: List.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. C. (1991). Somatic markers and the guidance of behavior: Theory and preliminary testing (pp. 217–229). In Levin, H. S., Eisenberg, H. M., & Benton, A. L. (Eds.), Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp. 230–255). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Daniels, D., & Plomin, R. (1985). Origins of individual differences in infant shyness. Developmental Psychology, 21, 118–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dannefer, D. (1989). Human action and its place in theories of aging. Journal of Aging and Health, 3, 1–20.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). Origin of species by means of natural selection. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, R. J. (2000). Affective style, psychopathology, and resilience: Brain mechanisms and plasticity. American Psychologist, 55(11), 1196–1214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. J. (2001). Toward a biology of personality and emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 935, 191–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, R. J., Kalin, N. H., & Shelton, S. E. (1993). Lateralized response to diazepam predicts temperamental style in rhesus monkeys. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 1106–1110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dawson, M. E., & Schell, A. M. (1982). Electrodermal responses to attended and nonattended significant stimuli during dichotic listening. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 315–324.Google ScholarPubMed
Araujo, I. E., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., & Hobden, P. (2003). Representation of umami taste in the human brain. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90(1), 313–319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCasper, A. J., & Carstens, A. A. (1981). Contingencies of stimulation: Effects of learning and emotion in neonates [Special issue: Variability in infancy]. Infant Behavior and Development, 4(1), 19–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
deCharms, R. (1976). Enhancing motivation: Change in the classroom. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
deCharms, R. (1979). Motivation in der Klasse [Motivation in the classroom]. Munich, Germany: MVG.Google Scholar
deCharms, R., & Moeller, G. H. (1962). Values expressed in American children's readers: 1800–1950. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64, 136–142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
deCharms, R., Morrison, H. W., Reitman, W., & McClelland, D. C. (1955). Behavioral correlates of directly and indirectly measured achievement motivation. In McClelland, D. C. (Ed.), Studies in motivation (pp. 414–423). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1980). The empirical exploration of intrinsic motivational processes. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 39–80). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dreu, C. K. W., Carnevale, P. J. D., Emans, B. J. M., & Vliert, E. (1994). Effects of gain-loss frames in negotiation: Loss aversion, mismatching, and frame adoption. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 60, 90–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deese, J., & Carpenter, J. A. (1951). Drive level and reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42, 236–238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fave, Delle A., & Bassi, M. (2000). The quality of experience in adolescents' daily lives: Developmental perspectives. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 126, 347–367.Google Scholar
Delville, Y., DeVries, G. J., & Ferris, C. F. (2000). Neural connections of the anterior hypothalamus and agonistic behavior in golden hamsters. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 55, 53–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dembo, T. (1931). Der Ärger als dynamisches Problem [Anger as a dynamic problem]. Psychologische Forschung, 15, 1–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dempster, F. N. (1985). Proactive interference in sentence recall: Topic-similarity effects and individual differences. Memory and Cognition, 13, 81–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. A., Luciana, M., Arbisi, P., Collins, P., & Leon, A. (1994). Dopamine and the structure of personality: Relation of agonist-induced dopamine activity to positive emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(3), 485–498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. A., & Morrone-Strupinsky, J. V. (2005). A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: Implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(3), 313–350; discussion 350–395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1991). The adaptive base of the neural hierarchy: Elementary motivational controls on network function. In Dienstbier, R. A. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 289–342). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Devine, P. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devinsky, O. (2000). Right cerebral hemisphere dominance for a sense of corporeal and emotional self. Epilepsy & Behavior, 1, 60–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dibbelt, S. (1997). Wechseln und Beibehalten von Zielen als Subfunktionen der Handlungskontrolle [Changing and keeping goals as subfunctions of action control]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Osnabrück, Germany.Google Scholar
Dickhäuser, O., & Galfe, E. (2004). Besser als …, schlechter als … – Leistungsbezogene Vergleichsprozesse in der Grundschule [Better …, worse … – Achievement-related comparison processes in elementary school]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 36, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickhäuser, O., & Rheinberg, F. (2003). Bezugsnormorientierung: Erfassung, Probleme, Perspektiven [Reference norm orientations: Measurement, problems, perspectives]. In Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. & Rheinberg, F. (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept (pp. 41–55). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Dickhäuser, O., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2000). Geschlechtsunterschiede im Lern- und Leistungsverhalten am Computer: Ein theoretischer Rahmen [Gender differences in learning and achievement behavior on the computer: A theoretical framework]. In Försterling, F., Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Silny, L. (Eds.), Kognitive und emotionale Aspekte der Motivation (pp. 53–76). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Dickhäuser, O., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2002). Erlernte Hilflosigkeit am Computer? Geschlechtsunterschiede in computerspezifischen Attributionen [Learned helplessness at the computer? Gender differences in computer-specific attributions]. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 49, 44–55.Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. (1997). Bolles's psychological syllogism. In Bouton, M. E. & Fanselow, M. S. (Eds.), Learning, motivation and cognition (pp. 345–367). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Dickinson, A., & Shanks, D. (1995). Instrumental action and causal representation. In Sperber, D., Premack, D., & Premack, A. James (Eds.), A causal cognition (pp. 3–25). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Diefendorff, J. M., Hall, R. J., Lord, R. G., & Strean, M. L. (2000). Action-state orientation: Construct validity of a revised measure and its relationship to work-related variables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 250–263.Google ScholarPubMed
Diener, C. I., & Dweck, C. S. (1978). An analysis of learned helplessness: Continuous changes in performance, strategy, and achievement cognitions following failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 451–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Implications for mental and physical health. Psychological Review, 96, 84–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dilthey, W. (1894). Ideen über eine beschreibende und zergliedernde Psychologie [Ideas on a descriptive and analytical psychology]. In Sitzungsberichte der Königlichen Preußischen Akademie der Wissenschaften zu Berlin (Phil. hist. Classe) (pp. 1309–1407). Berlin, Germany: LIII.Google Scholar
DiVitto, B., & McArthur, L. Z. (1978). Developmental differences in the use of distinctiveness, consensus, and consistency information for making causal attributions. Developmental Psychology, 14, 474–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dix, T., Ruble, D. N., & Zambarano, R, (1989). Mothers' implicit theories of discipline: Child effects, parent effects, and the attribution process. Child Development, 60, 1373–1391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A. (1993). Social-cognitive mechanism in the development of conduct disorder and depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 559–584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A., & Colie, J. D. (1987). Social information processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children's peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1146–1158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollard, J., Doob, L., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, H. O., & Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustration and aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality and psychotherapy: An analysis in terms of learning, thinking, and culture. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Domjan, M., Blesbois, E., & Williams, J. (1998). The adaptive significance of sexual conditioning: Pavlovian control of sperm release. Psychological Science, 9, 411–415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donders, F. C. (1862). Die Schnelligkeit psychischer Prozesse [The speed of mental processes]. Archiv für Anatomie und Physiologie, 657–681.Google Scholar
Donley, R. E., & Winter, D. G. (1970). Measuring the motives of public officials at a distance: An exploratory study of American presidents. Behavioral Sciences, 15, 227–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, M. (1986). How institutions think. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.Google Scholar
Droege, K. L., & Stipek, D. J. (1993). Children's use of dispositions to predict classmates' behavior. Developmental Psychology, 29, 646–654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, E. (1932). The relationship between muscular tension and quality of performance. American Journal of Psychology, 44, 535–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, E. (1934). Emotion: An example of the need for reorientation in psychology. Psychological Review, 41, 184–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, E. (1941). An explanation of “emotional” phenomena without the use of the concept “emotion.” Journal of General Psychology, 25, 283–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, E. (1957). The psychological significance of the concept of “arousal” or “activation.” Psychological Review, 64, 265–275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duffy, E. (1962). Activation and behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Düker, H. (1931). Psychologische Unter suchungen über freie und zwangsläufige Arbeitsweise: Experimentelle Beiträge zur Willens- und Arbeitspsychologie [Psy chological studies of free and inevitable work: Experimental contributions on the psychology of volition and work]. Leipzig, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
Düker, H. (1975). Untersuchungen über die Ausbildung des Wollens [Studies on the development of volition]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (1975). The role of expectations and attributions in the alleviation of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 674–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (2002). The development of ability conceptions. In Wigfield, A. & Eccles, J. S. (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 57–88). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (2003). Clarifying achievement goals and their impact. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 541–553.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S., & Elliott, E. S. (1983). Achievement motivation. In Hetherington, E. M. (Ed.), Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 643–691). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. I. (1973). Learned helplessness and reinforcement responsibility in children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 25, 109–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, F. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, C. S., & Repucci, N. D. (1973). Learned helplessness and reinforcement responsibility in children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 25, 109–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior. American Psychologist, 54, 408–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eaves, G., & Rush, A. J. (1984). Cognitive patterns in symptomatic and remitted unipolar major depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 31–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ebbinghaus, H. (1902). Abriß der Psychologie [Summary of psychology]. Leipzig, Germany: Veit.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (1984). Sex differences in achievement patterns. In Sonderegger, T. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium of Motivation: Vol. 32 (pp. 97–132). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (1987). Gender roles and women's achievement-related decisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 135–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (2005). Subjective task value and the Eccles et al. model of achievement related choices. In Elliot, A. J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 105–121). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S., Adler, T. F., & Meece, J. L. (1984). Sex differences in achievement: A test of alternate theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(1), 26–43.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S., Barber, B., & Jozefowicz, D. (1999). Linking gender to educational, occupational and recreational choices: Applying the Eccles model of achievement related choices. In Swann, W. B. & Langlois, J. H. (Eds.), Sexism and stereotypes in modern society (pp. 153–192). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1995). In the mind of the actor: The structure of adolescents' achievement task values and expectancy-related beliefs. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(3), 215–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 109–132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Harold, R., & Blumenfeld, P. B. (1993). Age and gender differences in children's self- and task perceptions during elementary school. Child Development, 64, 830–847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (Volume Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3, 5th ed., pp. 1017–1095). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Eckhardt, G. (1968). Die entwicklungspsychologische Abhängigkeit der Konfliktreaktion vom Grad der Misserfolgswahrscheinlichkeit [A developmental perspective the dependence of conflict responses on the likelihood of failure]. Unpublished pre-Diplom thesis, University of Münster, Germany.
Edwards, W. (1954). The theory of decision-making. Psychological Bulletin, 51, 380–417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, W. (1962). Utility, subjective probability, their interaction, and variance preferences. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 6, 42–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S. C. (2002). Predictive validity of an Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1441–1455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrlich, D., Guttmann, I., Schönbach, P., & Mills, J. (1957). Postdecision exposure to relevant information. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 98–102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1973). Der vorprogrammierte Mensch [The preprogrammed human]. Vienna, Austria: Molden.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1975). Krieg und Frieden aus der Sicht der Verhaltensforschung [The biology of peace and war: Men, animals, and aggression]. Munich, Germany: Piper.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1984). Die Biologie des menschlichen Verhaltens: Grundriß der Humanethologie [The biology of human behavior: Outline of human ethology]. Munich, Germany: Piper.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1988). Der Mensch – das riskierte Wesen: Zur Naturgeschichte menschlicher Unvernunft [On the natural history of human folly]. Munich, Germany: Piper.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1997). Die Biologie menschlichen Verhaltens – Grundriss der Humanethologie [Human ethology – Foundations of human behavior]. Munich, Germany: Piper.Google Scholar
Eimer, M. (1987). Konzepte von Kausalität [Concepts of causality]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I., Liebermann, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1996). Detrimental effects of reward: Reality or myth?American Psychologist, 51, 1153–1166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1998). Reward, intrinsic interest, and creativity: New findings. American Psychologist, 53, 676–679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in the facial expressions of emotion. In Cole, J. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1971 (pp. 207–283). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 124–129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elias, N. (1969). Über den Prozess der Zivilisation: Soziogenetische und psychogenetische Untersuchungen [On the process of civilization: Sociogenetic and psychogenetic investigations]. Berne, Switzerland: Francke.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. (1997). Integrating the “classic” and “contemporary” approaches to achievement motivation: A hierarchical model of achievement motivation. In Maehr, M. & Pintrich, P. (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10, pp. 243–279). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, 169–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliot, A. (2005). A conceptual history of the achievement goal construct. In Elliot, A. J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 52–72). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 218–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. (1996). Approach and avoidance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 461–475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2×2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501–519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliot, E. S., & Dweck, C. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 5–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, M. H. (1928). The effect of change of reward on the maze performance of rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 19–30.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An approach to personality and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1058–1068.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (1992). Abstract versus concrete goals: Personal striving level, physical illness and psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 292–300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emmons, R. A. (2003). Personal goals, life meaning and virtue: Wellsprings of a positive life. In Keyes, C. L. & Haidt, J. (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 105–128). Washington, DC: American Psychological Associaton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmons, R. A., & McAdams, D. (1991). Personal strivings and motive dispositions: Exploring the links. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 648–654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engeser, S. (2004). Lernmotivation und volitionale Handlungssteuerung: Eine Längsschnittstudie beim Statistik Lernen im Psychologiestudium [Learning motivation and volitional action regulation: A longitudinal study on learning elementary statistics]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Potsdam, Germany.Google Scholar
Engeser, S., Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Bischoff, J. (2005). Motivation, Flow-Erleben und Lernleistung in universitären Lernsettings [Motivation, flow-experience, and performance in learning settings at universities]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 19, 159–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Entin, E. E. (1974). Effects of achievement-oriented and affiliative motives on private and public performance. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 219–236). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Entwisle, D. R. (1972). To dispel fantasies about fantasy-based measures of achievement motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 377–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Entwistle, D., & Hayduk, L. (1978). Too great expectations: Young children's academic outlook. Baltimore: The John Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Epstein, L. H., Truesdale, R., Wojcik, A., Paluch, R. A., & Raynor, H. A. (2003). Effects of deprivation on hedonics and reinforcing value of food. Physiology & Behavior, 78, 221–227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, S. (1962). The measurement of drive and conflict in humans: Theory and experiment. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1962 (pp. 127–206). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709–724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 390–405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.). (1996). The road to excellence. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society (Rev. ed.). New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Eron, L. D. (1994). Theories of aggression. From drives to cognitions. In Huesmann, L. R. (Ed.), Aggressive behaviour: Current perspectives (pp. 3–11). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Escalona, S. K. (1940). The effect of success and failure upon the level of aspiration and behavior in manic-depressive psychoses. University of Iowa Studies: Child Welfare, 16, 199–302.Google Scholar
Estes, W. K. (1958). Stimulus-response theory of drive. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1958 (pp. 35–69). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Everitt, B. J. (1990). Sexual motivation: A neural and behavioural analysis of the mechanisms underlying appetitive and copulatory responses of male rats. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 14(2), 217–232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Exline, R. V. (1962). Need affiliation and initial communication behavior in problem solving groups characterized by low interpersonal visibility. Psychological Reports, 10, 79–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of personality. In Pervin, L. (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 244–276). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Falbo, T. (1975). Achievement attributions of kindergarteners. Developmental Psychology, 11, 529–530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fallo-Mitchell, L., & Ryff, C. D. (1982). Preferred timing of female life events. Research on Aging, 4, 249–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fajans, S. (1933). Die Bedeutung der Entfernung für die Stärke eines Aufforderungscharakters beim Säugling und Kleinkind [The meaning of distance for the degree of variance as experienced by the infant]. Psychologische Forschung, 17, 215–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farr, J. L., Hofmann, D. A., & Mathieu, J. E. (1993). Job perception, job satisfaction relations: An empirical comparison of three competing theories. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision, 56(3), 370–387.Google Scholar
Fazey, J. A., & Hardy, L. (1988). The inverted-U hypothesis: A catastrophe for sport psychology. In British Association for Sports Sciences Monograph no. 1. Leeds, England.
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feather, N. T. (1959a). Subjective probability and decision under uncertainty. Psychological Review, 66, 150–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1959b). Success probability and choice behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 257–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1961). The relationship of persistence at a task to expectation of success and achievement related motives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 552–561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1962). The study of persistence. Psychological Bulletin, 59, 94–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1963b). The relationship of expectation of success to reporter probability, task structure and achievement-related motivation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 231–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1967). Valence of outcome and expectation of success in relation to task difficulty and perceived locus of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 372–386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feather, N. T. (Ed.). (1982). Expectations and actions: Expectancy-value models in psychology.Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Feld, S. C. (1967). Longitudinal study of the origins of achievement strivings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 408–414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, N. S., Higgins, E. T., Karlovac, M., & Ruble, D. N. (1976). Use of consensus information in causal attribution as a function of temporal presentation and availability of direct information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 694–698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenz, W. D. (1975). Strategies for coping with stress. In Sarason, I. G. & Spielberger, C. D. (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 2, pp. 305–336). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Ferguson, E. D. (1962). Ego involvement: A critical examination of some methodological issues. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64, 407–417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feshbach, S., & Singer, R. D. (1971). Television and aggression: An experimental field study.San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1942). A theoretical interpretation of shifts in level of aspiration. Psychological Review, 49, 235–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1964). Conflict, decision, and dissonance.Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 203–210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956). When prophecy fails.Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K. (1982). Causal schemata: Review and criticism of research on a popular construct. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 1001–1013.Google Scholar
Fiedler, K., & Bless, H. (2002). Soziale Kognition [Social cognition]. In Stroebe, W., Jonas, K., & Hewstone, M. (Eds.), Sozialpsychologie (4th ed., pp. 125–164). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fincham, F., & Jaspars, J. (1979). Attribution of responsibility to the self and other in children and adults. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1589–1602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fineman, S. (1977). The achievement motive construct and its measurement: Where are we now?British Journal of Psychology, 68, 1–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finkel, E. J., & Campbell, W. K. (2001). Self-control and accommodation in close relationships: An interdependence analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 265–277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkelstein, N. W., & Ramey, C. T. (1977). Learning to control the environment in infancy. Child Development, 48, 806–819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisch, R. (1970). Konfliktmotivation und Examen [Conflict motivation and examinations]. Meisenheim, Germany: Hain.Google Scholar
Fishbach, A. U., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not into temptations: Momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 296–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fishman, D. B. (1966). Need and expectancy as determinants of affiliative behavior in small groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 155–164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, D. W., & Maddi, S. R. (1961). A conceptual framework. In Fiske, D. W. & Maddi, S. R. (Eds.), Functions of varied experience (pp. 11–56). Homewood, IL: Dorsey.Google Scholar
Fitch, G. (1970). Effects of self-esteem, perceived performance, and choice on causal attributions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 311–315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance.Oxford, UK: Brooks(Cole.Google Scholar
Flammer, A., Züblin, C., & Grob, A. (1988). Sekundäre Kontrolle bei Jugendlichen [Secondary control in adolescents]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 20, 239–262.Google Scholar
Fleming, A. S., Corter, C., Franks, P., Surbey, M., Schneider, B., & Steiner, M. (1993). Postpartum factors related to mother's attraction to newborn infant odors. Developmental Psychobiology, 26(2), 115–132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, E. M. (1984). The power motive and reactivity to power stresses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 853–859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, E. M. (1985). The power motive. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1408–1416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, E. M., & Carver, R. A. (2000). Achievement and power motives, performance feedback, and creativity. Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 380–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, E. M., & Farrow, D. L. (1979). The power motive as an influence on use of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2091–2097.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, E. M., & Smith, T. (1982). The power motive as an influence on group decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 178–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. A. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-age community sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 21, 219–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). Ways of coping questionnaire: Manual.Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Forrest, D. W. (1959). The role of muscular tension in the recall of interrupted tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 181–184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Förster, J., & Liberman, N. (2002, June). Introducing a motivational priming model. Paper presented at the 13th general meeting of the European Association of Experimental Social Psychology, San Sebastian, Spain.Google Scholar
Försterling, F. (1989). Models of covariation and attribution: How do they relate to the analogy of analysis of variance?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(4), 615–625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Försterling, F. (2001). Attribution: An introduction to theories, research and applications. Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Försterling, F., Bühner, F., & Gall, S. (1998). Attributions of depressed persons: How consistant are they with the covariation principle?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 1047–1061.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ford, M. E., & Nicholls, C. W. (1987). A taxonomy of human goals and some possible applications. In Ford, M. E. & Ford, D. H. (Eds.), Humans as self-constructing systems: Putting the framework to work (pp. 289–311). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Forrest, D. W. (1959). The role of muscular tension in the recall of interrupted tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 181–184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, H. (1971). Implications of sensory reinforcement. In Glaser, R. (Ed.), The nature of reinforcement (pp. 151–195). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1988). Patterns of brain electrical activity during facial signs of emotion in 10-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 24, 230–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, K., Goudas, M, Biddle, S., Duda, J. L., & Armstrong, N. (1994). Children's task and ego goal profiles in sport. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, 253–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, J. D. (1935). Individual differences in certain aspects of the level of aspiration. American Journal of Psychology, 47, 119–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freedman, J. L. (1965) Preference for dissonant information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 287–289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, G. L. (1930). Changes in tonus during completed and interrupted mental work. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 4, 309–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freitas, A. L., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2002). Regulatory fit and temptation during goal pursuit. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 291–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
French, E. G. (1955). Some characteristics of achievement motivation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 232–236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, E. G. (1956). Motivation as a variable in workpartner selection. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53, 96–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, E. G. (1958a). Development of a measure of complex motivation. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 242–248). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
French, E. G. (1958b). Effects of the interaction of motivation and feedback on task performance. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 400–408). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
French, E. G., & Chadwick, I. (1956). Some characteristics of affiliation motivation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 296–300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, E. G., & Lesser, G. S. (1964). Some characteristics of the achievement motive in women. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 119–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, J. A., Kamil, A. C., & Leger, D. (Eds.). (2001). Evolutionary psychology and motivation. In Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 47 (p. 221). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
French, J. R. P., & Raven B. H. (1959). The basis of social power. In Cartwright, D. (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 150–167). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1895). Entwurf einer Psychologie [Outline of a psychological science]. In: Freud, S. (Ed.), From the beginnings of psychology (pp. 370–466). London: Imago Publishing.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1901). Zur Psychopathologie des Alltagslebens [The psychopathology of everyday life]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. IV (1952). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1915). Triebe und Triebschicksale [Drives and their fate]. Gesammelte Werke, Bd. X. Frankfurt: Fischer, 1952.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1926). Hemmung, Symptom, Angst [Inhibition, symptom, anxiety]. Gesammelte Werke, Bd. XIV. Frankfurt: Fischer, 1952.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1952). Die Abwehrneuropsychosen [The neuropsychoses of defense]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. I (1894). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1952). Die Traumdeutung [The interpretation of dreams]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. II–III (1900). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1952). Hemmung, Symptom, Angst [Inhibition, symptoms and anxiety]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. ⅪV (1926). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1952). Triebe und Triebschicksale [Instincts and their vicissitudes]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. X (1915). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1961). Drei Abhandlungen zur Sexualtheorie [Three essays on the theory of sexuality]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. V (3rd ed.) (1905). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1963). Das Unbehagen in der Kultur [Civilization and its discontents]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. ⅪV (3rd ed.) (1930). Frankfurt, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). The orchestration of selection, optimization and compensation: An action-theoretical conceptualization of a theory of developmental regulation. In Perrig, W. J. & Grob, A. (Eds.), Control of human behavior, mental processes, and consciousness: Essays in honor of the 60th birthday of August Flammer (pp. 35–58). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Frey, D. (1981). Informationssuche und Informationsbewertung bei Entscheidungen [The search for and evaluation of information in decision making]. Stuttgart, Germany: Huber.Google Scholar
Frey, D., & Irle, M. (1972). Some conditions to produce a dissonance and an incentive effect in a “forced-compliance” situation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 45–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frey, K. S., & Ruble, D. N. (1985). What children say when the teacher is not around: Conflicting goals in social comparison and performance assessment in the classroom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 550–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frey, R. S. (1984). Does n-achievement cause economic development? A cross-lagged panel analysis of the McClelland thesis. Journal of Social Psychology, 122, 67–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, J. M., & Halaas, J. L. (1998). Leptin and the regulation of body weight in mammals. Nature, 395(6704), 763–770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fries, S. (2002). Wollen und Können [Volition and ability]. Münster, Germany: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Fries, S., Lund, B., & Rheinberg, F. (1999). Lässt sich das Training induktiven Denkens durch gleichzeitige Motivförderung optimieren? [Does simultaneous motive modification optimise the teaching of inductive reasoning?]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 13, 37–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43, 249–358.CrossRef
Fuchs, R. (1954). Gewißheit, Motivation und bedingter Reflex [Certainty, motivation, and conditioned reflex]. Meisenheim, Germany: Hain.Google Scholar
Fuhrmann, A., & Kuhl, J. (1998). Maintaining a healthy diet: Effects of personality and self-reward versus self-punishment on commitment to and enactment of self-chosen and assigned goals. Psychology and Health, 13, 651–686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuster, J. M. (2001). The prefrontal cortex – an update: Time is of the essence. Neuron, 30(2), 319–333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fyans, L. J., Salili, M., Maehr, M. L., & Desai, K. A. (1983). A cross-cultural exploration into the meaning of achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1000–1013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gagné, F. M., & Lydon, J. E. (2001a). Mind-set and close relationships: When bias leads to (in)accurate predictions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 85–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gagné, F. M., & Lydon, J. E. (2001b). Mindset and relationship illusions: The moderating effects of domain specificity and relationship commitment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1144–1155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gagné, F. M., Lydon, J. E., & Bartz, J. A. (2003). Effects of mindset on the predictive validity of relationship constructs. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 35, 292–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galbraith, J., & Cummings, L. (1967). An empiric investigation of the motivational determinants of past performance: Interactive effects between instrumentality-valence, motivation, and ability. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 2, 237–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Magee, J. C. (2003). From power to action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 453–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Garland, H. (1984). Relation of effort-performance expectancy to performance in goal-setting experiments. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 79–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaugele, H., & Ullmer, C. (1990). Zur Anreizstruktur des Bodybuildings [The incentive structure of body building]. Unpublished manuscript, University of Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Gavin, J. F. (1970). Ability, effort, and role perception as antecedents of job performance. Experimental Publication System (Manuscript no. 190A), 5.Google Scholar
Gawrilow, C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2004, April). Inhibition fortlaufender Reaktionen durch Vorsätze bei ADHS Kindern [Inhibition of continuous reactions in ADHD children]. Paper presented at the symposium “Recent developments in research on implementation intentions” at the 46th meeting of the Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen (TeaP), Giessen, Germany.Google Scholar
Gebhard, M. (1948). Effects of success and failure upon the attractiveness of activities as a function of experience, expectation, and need. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 371–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gehlen, A. (1958). Der Mensch: Seine Natur und seine Stellung in der Welt [The human being: His nature and status in the world]. Bonn, Germany: Athenäum.Google Scholar
Georgopolous, B. S., Mahony, G. M., & Jones, N. W. (1957). A path-goal approach to productivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 41, 345–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geppert, U., & Gartmann, D. (1983, July). The emergence of self evaluative emotions as consequences of achievement actions. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Munich, Germany.Google Scholar
Geppert, U., & Heckhausen, H. (1990). Ontogenese der Emotion [Ontogenesis of emotion]. In Scherer, K. R. (Ed.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Psychologie der Emotion (Vol. IV, No. 3, pp. 115–213). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Geppert, U., & Küster, U. (1983). The emergence of “wanting to do it oneself”: A precursor of achievement motivation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 6, 355–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gianotti, M., Roca, P., & Palou, A. (1988). Body weight and tissue composition in rats made obese by a cafeteria diet: Effect of 24 hours starvation. Hormone and Metabolic Research, 20(4), 208–212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Adaptive thinking: Rationality in the real world. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & the ABC Research Group. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gilligan, S. G. (1997). The courage to love: Principles and practices of self-relations psychotherapy.New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Gjesme, T. (1971). Motive to achieve success and motive to avoid failure in relation to school performance for pupils of different ability levels. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 15, 81–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gjesme, T., & Nygard, R. (1970). Achievement-related motives: Theoretical considerations and construction of a measuring instrument. Unpublished manuscript, University of Oslo, Norway.Google Scholar
Glixman, A. F. (1948). An analysis of the use of the interruption-technique in experimental studies of “repression.”Psychological Bulletin, 45, 491–506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, L. R. (1982). From ace to zombie: Some explorations in the language of personality. In Spielberger, C. D. & Butcher, J. N. (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 1, pp. 203–234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goleman, D. (1994). Emotional intelligence. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Suchen, Finden und Festigen der eigenen Identität: Unstillbare Zielintentionen [Searching, finding, and consolidating of one's own identity: Unsaturatable goal intentions]. In Heckhausen, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Weinert, F. E. (Eds.), Jenseits des Rubikon: Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften (pp. 176–189). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 53–92). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1991). Abwägen und Planen [Consideration and planning]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: the role of intentions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 141–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1998). Implicit and explicit processes in goal pursuit. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society of Experimental Social Psychology, Lexington, KY.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 186–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (2003). Why we thought that action mind-sets affect illusions of control. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 261–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.). (1996). The psychology of action:Linking cognition and motivation to behavior. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bayer, U. (1999). Deliberative versus implemental mindsets in the control of action. In Chaiken, S. & Trope, Y. (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 403–422). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bayer, U. (2000). Becoming a better person without changing yourself. Paper presented at the Self and Identity Pre-conference of the annual meeting of the Society of Experimental Social Psychology, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Bayer, U., & McCulloch, C. (2003). The control of the unwanted. In Hassin, R., Uleman, J., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), The new unconscious (pp. 485–515). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Bayer, U., Steller, B., & Bargh, J. A. (2002). Delegating control to the environment: Perception, attention and memory for preselected behavioral cues. Unpublished manuscript, University of Konstanz, Germany.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 186–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Fujita, K., & Oettingen, G. (2004). Planning and the implementation of goals. In Baumeister, R. & Vohs, K. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation(pp. 211–228). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Ratajczak, H. (1990). From weighing to willing: Approaching a change decision through pre- or postdecisional mentation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 45, 41–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Steller, B. (1990). Deliberative and implemental mind-sets: Cognitive tuning toward congruous thoughts and information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1119–1127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kirchhof, O. (1998). The willful pursuit of identity. In Heckhausen, J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulation across the life span (pp. 389–423). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kinney, R. F. (1989). Effects of deliberative and implemental mind-sets on illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 531–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, G. B. (1996). Goal effects on action and cognition. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 361–399). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Schaal, B. (1998). Metacognition in action: The importance of implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 124–136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollwitzer, P. M., Trötschel, R., Bayer, U., & Sumner, M. (2004). Potential costs of self-regulation by implementation intentions: Rebound and ego-depletion effects? Unpublished manuscript, University of Konstanz, Germany.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Wicklund, R. A. (1985a). Self-symbolizing and the neglect of others' perspectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 702–715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Wicklund, A. (1985b). The pursuit of self-defining goals. Action control: from cognition to behavior (pp. 61–85). Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
Goodenough, F. L. (1931). Anger in young children. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, P. S., Rose, J. H., & Furcon, J. E. (1970). Comparison of motivational antecedents of the work performance of scientists and engineers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 54, 491–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodstadt, B. E., & Hjelle, L. A. (1973). Power to the powerless: Locus of control and the use of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 190–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goschke, T. (1997a). Zur Funktionsanalyse des Willens: Integration kognitions-, motivations- und neuropsychologischer Perspektiven [On the functional architecture of volitional action: Integration of cognitive, motivational, and neuropsychological perspectives]. Psychologische Beiträge, 39, 375–412.Google Scholar
Goschke, T. (1997b). Implicit learning of perceptual and motor sequences: Evidence for independent learning systems. In Stadler, M. & French, P. (Eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 401–444). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (1993). The representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1211–1226.Google Scholar
Gottfredson, L. S. (1981). Circumscription and compromise: A developmental theory of occupational aspirations. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 28(6), 545–579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behavioral development theory. Developmental Psychology, 27, 4–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottschaldt, K. (1926). Über den Einfluss der Erfahrung auf die Wahrnehmung von Figuren [On the influence of past experience on perception]. Psychologische Forschung, 8, 261–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gough, H. G. (1990). The California Psychological Inventory. In Watkins, C. E. & Campbell, V. L. (Eds.), Testing in counselling practice (pp. 37–62). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Graen, G. (1969). Instrumentality theory of work motivation: Some experimental results and suggested modifications. Journal of Applied Psychology Monographs, 53, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J. M., & Desjardins, C. (1980). Classical conditioning: Induction of luteinizing hormone and testosterone secretion in anticipation of sexual activity. Science, 210, 1039–1041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, S., & Hudley, C. (1994). Attributions of aggressive and nonaggressive African-American male early adolescents: A study of construct accessibility. Developmental Psychology, 30, 365–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, S., Hudley, C., & Williams, E. (1992). Attributional and emotional determinants of aggression among African-American and Latino young adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 28, 731–740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, S., Weiner, B., & Benesh-Weiner, M. (1995). An attributional analysis of the development of excuse giving in aggressive and nonaggressive African American boys. Developmental Psychology, 31(2), 274–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1971). The psychology of fear and stress. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck's theory of personality. In Eysenck, H. J. (Ed.), A model for personality (pp. 246–276). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1982). The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Graziano, W. G., & Eisenberg, N. H. (1997). Agreeableness: A dimension of personality. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 793–825). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Green, D. (1963). Volunteering and the recall of interrupted tasks. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 397–401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. M., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., & Maynard, A. (2002). Cultural pathways through universal development. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 461–490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenough, A., Cole, G., Lewis, J., Lockton, A., & Blundell, J. (1998). Untangling the effects of hunger, anxiety, and nausea on energy intake during intravenous cholecystokinin octapeptide (cck-8) infusion. Physiology & Behavior, 65(2), 303–310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G. (1982). Ego task analysis: An integration of research on ego-involvement and self-awareness. In Hastorf, A. & Isen, A. (Eds.), Cognitive social psychology (pp. 109–147). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109, 3–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greve, W., & Wentura, D. (2003). Immunizing the self: Self-concept stabilization through reality-adaptive self-definitions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 39–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grinker, J. (1969). Cognitive control of classical eyelid conditioning. In Zimbardo, P. G. (Ed.), The cognitive control of motivation (pp. 126–135). Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
Grob, A., Krings, F., & Bangerter, A. (2001). Life markers in biographical narratives of people from three cohorts: A life span perspective in its historical context. Human Development, 44(4), 171–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Groeben, N., Wahl, D., Schlee, J., & Scheele, B. (1988). Das Forschungsprogramm Subjektive Theorien. Eine Einführung in die Psychologie des reflexiven Subjekts [Research on subjective theories. An introduction to the psychology of the reflexive subject]. Tübingen, Germany: Francke.Google Scholar
Groos, K. (1899). Die Spiele des Menschen [The play of man]. Jena, Germany: Fischer.
Gubler, H., Paffrath, M., & Bischof, N. (1994). Untersuchungen zur Systemanalyse der sozialen Motivation III: Eine Aestimationsstudie zur Sicherheits- und Erregungsregulation während der Adoleszenz [An estimation study of security and arousal regulation during adolescence]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 202, 95–132.Google Scholar
Guevara, M. L. (1994). Alienation und Selbstkontrolle: Das Ignorieren eigener Gefühle [Alienation and self-control: Ignoring one's own feelings]. Berne, Switzerland: Lang.Google Scholar
Guinote, A., Judd, C. M., & Brauer, M. (2002). Effects of power on perceived and objective group variability: Evidence that more powerful groups are more variable. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 708–721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gupta, B. S., & Nagpal, M. (1978). Impulsivity(sociability and reinforcement in verbal operant conditioning. British Journal of Psychology, 69, 203–206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurack, E. (1978). Die Entwicklung des Fähigkeitskonzepts im Vorschulalter [The development of the ability concept at preschool age]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Haase, C. M., Heckhausen, J., & Köller, O. (2007). Goal engagement during the school-work transition: Beneficial for all, particularly for girls. Journal of Research on Adolescence.Google Scholar
Haber, R. N. (1958). Discrepancy from adaptation level as a source of affect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 370–375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haber, R. N., & Alpert, R. (1958). The role of situation and picture cues in projective measurement of the achievement motive. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 644–663). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Hackman, J. R., & Porter, L. W. (1968). Expectancy theory predictions of work effectiveness. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 417–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haffner, S. (2003). Historische Variationen [Historical variations]. Munich, Germany: dtv.Google Scholar
Haider, M. (1969). Elektrophysiologische Indikatoren der Aktiviertheit [Electrophysiological indicators of activation]. In Schönpflug, W. (Ed.), Methoden der Aktivierungsforschung (pp. 125–156). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Halisch, C., & Halisch, F. (1980). Kognitive Voraussetzungen frühkindlicher Selbstbewertungsreaktionen nach Erfolg und Misserfolg [Cognitive prerequisites of self-evaluative reactions after success and failure in early childhood]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 12, 193–212.Google Scholar
Halisch, F. (1986). Operante und respondente Verfahren zur Messung des Leistungsmotivs [Operant and respondent measures for the assessment of need for achievement]. Munich, Germany: Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research.Google Scholar
Halisch, F., & Geppert, U. (2001a). Motives, personal goals, and life satisfaction in old age: First results from the Munich Twin Study (GOLD). In Trends and prospects in motivation research (pp. 389–409). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Halisch, F., & Geppert, U. (2001b). Genetic vs. environmental determinants of traits, motives, self-referential cognitions, and volitional control in old age: First results from the Munich Twin Study (GOLD). In Trends and prospects in motivation research (pp. 359–387). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Halisch, F., & Heckhausen, H. (1977). Search for feedback information and effort regulation during task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 724–733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halisch, F., & Heckhausen, H. (1988). Motive-dependent vs. ability-dependent valence functions for success and failure. In Halisch, F. & Bercken, J. (Eds.), Intentional perspectives on achievement and task motivation. Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Halisch, F., & Kuhl, J. (Eds.). (1986). Motivation, intention, and volition. Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
Hamburg, D. A. (1963). Emotions in the perspective of human evolution. In Knapp, P. H. (Ed.), Expression of emotions in man (pp. 300–317). New York: International University Press.Google Scholar
Hamilton, J. O. (1974). Motivation and risk-taking behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 856–864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, V. L. (1980). Intuitive psychologist or intuitive lawyer?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 767–772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 17–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, R. D., & Stonner, D. M. (1978). Attributes and attributions: Inferring stimulus properties, actors' dispositions, and causes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 657–667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., & Elliot, A. (1998). Rethinking achievement goals: When are they adaptive for college students and why?Educational Psychologist, 33(1), 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., & Elliot, A. (2002). Predicting succes in college: A longituinal study of achievement goals and ability measure as predictors of interest and performance from freshman year through graduation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 562–575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harackiewicz, J. M., & Elliot, A. (1993). Achievement goals and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 904–915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harcourt, A. H. (1987). Dominance and fertility among female primates. Journal of Zoology, 213, 471–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harcourt, A. H. (1989). Social influences on competitive ability: Alliances and their consequences. In Standen, V. & Foley, R. A. (Eds.), Comparative socioecology – The behavioural ecology of humans and other mammals (pp. 223–242). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hardy, K. R. (1957). Determinants of conformity and attitude change. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 289–294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardyck, J. A., & Braden, M. (1962). Prophecy fails again: A report of a failure to replicate. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 136–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1950). Learning and satiation of response in intrinsically motivated complex puzzle performance by monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 289–294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, H. F. (1953). Motivation as a factor in the acquisition of new responses. In Current theory and research in motivation: A symposium. (pp. 24–49). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1971). Learning to love.San Francisco: Abbion.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F., & Harlow, M. H. (1966). Learning how to care. American Scientist, 54, 244–272.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F., & Zimmermann, R. (1959). Affectional responses in the infant monkey. Science, 130, 421–432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmon-Jones, E. (2000). Cognitive dissonance and experienced negative affect: Evidence that dissonance increases experienced negative affect even in the absence of aversive consequences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1490–1501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2002). Testing the action-based model of cognitive dissonance: The effect of action-orientation on post-decisional attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 711–723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, B. (1979). Whatever happened to little Albert?American Psychologist, 34, 151–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child's environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102, 458–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harter, S. (1974). Pleasure derived from cognitive challenge and mastery. Child Development, 45, 661–669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered. Human Development, 21, 34–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartshorne, H., & May, M. A. (1928). Studies in the nature of character (Vol. 1). Studies in deceit.New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hartshorne, H., & May, M. A. (1929). Studies in the nature of character (Vol. 2). Studies in service and self-control.New York: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hautzinger, M. (1994). Action control in the context of psychopathological disorders. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 209–215). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Havighurst, R. J. (1953). Human development and education. London: Longmans.Google Scholar
Hayashi, T., & Yamauchi, K. (1964). The relation of children's need for achievement to their parents' home discipline in regard to independence and mastery. Bulletin of Kyoto Gakugei University, A25, 31–40.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1946). Onthe nature of fear. Psychological Review, 53, 259–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1953). Heredity and environment in mammalian behavior. British Journal of Animal Behavior, 1, 43–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1955). Drives and the C. N. S. (conceptual nervous system). Psychological Review, 62, 243–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1955). Motivationsanalyse der Anspruchsniveau-Setzung [Motivational analysis of the setting of levels of aspiration]. Psychologische Forschung, 25, 118–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1960). Die Problematik des Projektionsbegriffs und die Grundlagen und Grundannahmen des Thematischen Auffassungstests [The problems surrounding the concept of projection and the basic assumptions of the Thematic Apperception Test]. Psychologische Beiträge, 5, 53–80.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1963a). Hoffnung und Furcht in der Leistungsmotivation [Hope and fear in achievement motivation]. Meisenheim(Glan, Germany: Hain.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1963b). Eine Rahmentheorie der Motivation in zehn Thesen [A framework theory of motivation in ten theses]. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 10, 604–626.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1964a). Über die Zweckmäßigkeit einiger Situationsbedingungen bei der inhaltsanalytischen Erfassung der Motivation [On the suitability of some situational conditions in the content-based assessment of motivation]. Psychologische Forschung, 27, 244–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1964b). Entwurf einer Psychologie des Spielens. Psychologische Forschung, 27, 225–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1965). Leistungsmotivation [Achievement motivation]. In Thomae, H. (Ed.), Handbuch der Psychologie (Vol. II, pp. 602–702). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1967). The anatomy of achievement motivation.New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1968). Achievement motive research: Current problems and some contributions toward a general theory of motivation. In Arnold, W. J. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1968 (pp. 103–174). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1969). Allgemeine Psychologie in Experimenten [General psychology in experiments]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1972). Die Interaktion der Sozialisationsvariablen in der Genese des Leistungsmotivs [The interaction of socialization variables in the genesis of the achievement motive]. In Graumann, C. F. (Ed.), Handbuch der Psychologie (Vol. 7/2, pp. 955–1019). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1973). Intervening cognitions in motivation. In Berlyne, D. & Madsen, K. B. (eds.), Pleasure, reward, and preference (pp. 217–242). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1974a). Leistung und Chancengleichheit [Achievement and equality of opportunity]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1974b). Motivationsanalysen [Analyses of motivation]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1975a). Effort expenditure, aspiration level and self-evaluation before and after unexpected performance shifts. Unpublished manuscript, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1975b). Fear of failure as a self-reinforcing motive system. In Sarason, I. G. & Spielberger, C. (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. II, pp. 117–128). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1977a). Achievement motivation and its constructs: A cognitive model. Motivation and Emotion, 1(4), 283–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1977b). Motivation: Kognitionspsychologische Aufspaltung eines summarischen Konstrukts [Motivation: Splitting a summary construct along the lines of cognitive psychology]. Psychologische Rundschau, 28, 175–189.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1978). Selbstbewertung nach erwartungswidrigem Leistungsverlauf: Einfluß von Motiv, Kausalattribution und Zielsetzung [Self-evaluation after unexpected performances: Influence of motives, causal attributions, and goal selection]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 10, 191–216.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1980). Motivation und Handeln [Motivation and action]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1981). Neuere Entwicklungen in der Motivationsforschung [Recent developments in motivation research]. In Michaelis, W. (Ed.), Bericht über den 32. Kongreß der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie, Zürich 1980 (pp. 325–335). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1982). The development of achievement motivation. In Hartup, W. W. (Ed.), Review of child development research (pp. 600–668). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1983a). Motivationsmodelle: Fortschreitende Entfaltung und unbehobene Mängel [Models of motivation: Progressive development and remaining shortcomings]. In Hacker, W., Volpert, W., & Cranach, M. V. (Eds.), Kognitive und motivationale Aspekte der Handlung (pp. 9–17). Berlin, Germany: VEB Deutscher Verlag der Wissenschaften.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1983b). Entwicklungsschritte in der Kausalattribution von Handlungsergebnissen [Developmental stages in the causal attribution of action outcomes]. Kindliche Erklärungsmuster: Entwicklungspsychologische Beiträge zur Attributionsforschung, 1, 49–85.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1984a). Attibutionsmuster für Leistungsergebnisse: Individuelle Unterschiede, mögliche Arten und deren Genese [Attribution patterns for performance outcomes: Individual differences, possible types, and their development]. In Weinert, F. E. & Kluwe, R. H. (Eds.), Metakognition, Motivation und Lernen (pp. 133–164). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1984b). Emergent achievement behavior: Some early developments. In Nicholls, J. (Ed.), Advances in achievement motivation (pp. 1–32). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1987a). Wünschen – Wählen – Wollen [Wishing – weighing – willing]. In Heckhausen, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Weinert, F. E. (Eds.), Jenseits des Rubikon: Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften (pp. 3–9). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1987b). Perspektiven einer Psychologie des Wollens [Perspectives on a psychology of the will]. In Heckhausen, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Weinert, F. E. (Eds.), Jenseits des Rubikon: Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften (pp. 121–142). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1987c). Vorsatz, Wille und Bedürfnis: Lewins frühes Vermächtnis und ein zugeschütteter Rubikon [Intention, will, and need: Lewin's early legacy and a missed Rubicon]. In Heckhausen, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Weinert, F. E. (Eds.), Jenseits des Rubikon: Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften (pp. 86–96). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1989). Motivation und Handeln [Motivation and action] (2nd ed.). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and action. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents and cognitive functioning in motivational versus volitional states of mind. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 101–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Weinert, F. E. (Eds.). (1987). Jenseits des Rubikon: Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften [Beyond the Rubicon: The will in the behavioral and social sciences]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Halisch, F. (1986, July). “Operant” versus “respondent” motive measures: A problem of validity or of construct? Paper presented at the 21st International Congress of Applied Psychology, Jerusalem, Israel.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Kuhl, J. (1985). From wishes to action: The dead ends and short cuts on the long way to action. In Frese, M. & Sabini, L. (Eds.), Goal-directed behavior: Psychological theory and research on action (pp. 134–160, 367–395). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Meyer, W. U. (1972). Selbständigkeitserziehung und Leistungsmotiv [Raising children to be independent and the achievement motive]. Forschungsgemeinschaft, 4, 10–14.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Rheinberg, F. (1980). Lernmotivation im Unterricht, erneut betrachtet [A new look at motivation to learn in the classroom]. Unterrichtswissenschaft, 8, 7–47.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Roelofsen, I. (1962). Anfänge und Entwicklung der Leistungsmotivation: (I) Im Wetteifer des Kleinkindes [Origins and development of achievement motivation: (I) In the competition of infancy]. Psychologische Forschung, 26, 313–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H., Schmalt, H.-D., & Schneider, K. (1985). Achievement motivation in perspective. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Strang, H. (1988). Efficiency under maximal performance demands: Exertion control, an individual-difference variable?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 489–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Wagner, I. (1965). Anfänge der Entwicklung der Leistungsmotivation: (II) In der Zielsetzung des Kleinkindes. Zur Genese des Anspruchsniveaus [Origins and development of achievement motivation: (II) In the goal setting of infancy. Development of the level of aspiration]. Psychologische Forschung, 28, 179–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1987a). Balancing for weaknesses and challenging developmental potential: A longitudinal study of mother-infant dyads in apprenticeship interactions. Developmental Psychology, 23, 762–770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1987b). How do mothers know? Infants' chronological age or infants performance as determinants of adaptation in maternal instructions?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 43, 212–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1988). Becoming aware of ones competence in the second year: Developmental progression within the mother-child dyad. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11, 305–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1989). Normatives Entwicklungswissen als Bezugsrahmen zur (Re)Konstruktion der eigenen Biographie [Normative developmental knowledge as a framework for (re)constructing one's own biography]. In Alheit, P. & Hörning, E. (Eds.), Biographisches Wissen: Beiträge zu einer Theorie lebensgeschichtlicher Erfahrung (pp. 202–282). Frankfurt, Germany: Campus.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1990). Erwerb und Funktion normativer Vorstellungen über den Lebenslauf: Ein entwicklungspsychologischer Beitrag zur sozio-psychischen Konstruktion von Biographien [Acquisition and function of normative conceptions about the life course: A developmental psychology approach to the socio-psychological construction of biographies]. Kölner Zeitschrift für Soziologie und Sozialpsychologie, 31, 351–373.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1991). Adults' expectancies about development and its controllability: Enhancing self-efficacy by social comparisons. In Schwarzer, R. (Ed.), Self-efficacy: Thought control of action (pp. 107–126). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1993). The development of mastery and its perception within caretaker-child dyads. In Messer, D. (Ed.), Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and social processes (pp. 55–79). New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (1997). Developmental regulation across adulthood: Primary and secondary control of age-related challenges. Developmental Psychology, 33, 176–187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckhausen, J. (1999). Developmental regulation in adulthood: Age-normative and sociostructural constraints as adaptive challenges. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (2000a). Evolutionary perspectives on human motivation. American Behavioral Scientist, 43, 1015–1029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (2000b). Developmental regulation across the life span: An action-phase model of engagement and disengagement with developmental goals. In Heckhausen, J. (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 213–231). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (2002a). Transition from school to work: Societal opportunities and the potential for individual agency. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60, 173–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (2002b). Developmental regulation of life-course transitions: A control theory approach. In Pulkkinen, L. & Caspi, A. (Eds.), Paths to successful development: Personality in the life course (pp. 257–280). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (2005). Competence and motivation in adulthood and old age: Making the most of changing capacities and resources. In Elliot, A. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 240–256). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1991). Perceived controllability of expected psychological change across adulthood and old age. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 46, 165–173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckhausen, J., & Brim, O. G. (1997). Perceived problems for self and others: Self-protection by social downgrading throughout adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 12, 610–619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckhausen, J., Chang, E. S., Chen, C., & Greenberger, E. (2007). Optimistic goal selection and control striving during the transition after high school. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Heckhausen, J., Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Gains and losses in development throughout adulthood as perceived by different adult age groups. Developmental Psychology, 25, 109–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Dweck, C. S. (1998).(Eds.). Life span perspectives on motivation and control. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Farruggia, S. P. (2003). Developmental regulation across the life span: A control-theory approach and implications for secondary education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, Monograph series II: Psychological aspects of education – Current trends. Leicester, UK: British Psychological Society.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Hosenfeld, B. (1988, October). Lebensspannenentwicklung von normativen Vorstellungen über Lebensspannenentwicklung [Life-span development of normative conceptions about life-span development]. Paper presented at the 36th Congress of the Deutsche Gesellschaft für Psychologie, Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Krüger, J. (1993). Developmental expectations for the self and most other people: Age grading in three functions of social comparison. Developmental Psychology, 29, 539–548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1993). Optimisation by selection and compensation: Balancing primary and secondary control in life span development. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 16(2), 287–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 102(2), 284–304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1999a). The primacy of primary control is a human universal: A reply to Gould's critique of the life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 106(3), 605–609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1999b). Biological and societal canalizations and individuals' developmental goals. In Brandstädter, J. & Lerner, R. (Eds.), Action and self development: Theory and research through the life span (pp. 67–103). London: Sage.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, J., Schulz, R. & Wrosch, C. (1998). Developmental regulation in adulthood: Optimization in primary and secondary control – A multiscale questionnaire (OPS-Scales) [Technical report]. In Heckhausen, J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulation across the life span (pp. 50–77). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Tomasik, M. J. (2002). Get an apprenticeship before school is out: How German adolescents adjust vocational aspirations when getting close to a developmental deadline. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60, 199–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Fleeson, W. W. (2001). Developmental regulation before and after a developmental deadline: The sample case of “biological clock” for child-bearing. Psychology and Aging, 16, 400–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heider, F. (1946). Attitudes and cognitive organization. Journal of Psychology, 21, 107–112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations.New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heider, F. (1960). The Gestalt theory of motivation. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1960 (pp. 145–172). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard?Psychological Review, 106, 766–794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heinz, W. R. (1999). From education to work: Cross-national perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heinz, W. R., Krüger, H., Rettke, U., Wachtveitl, E., & Witzel, A. (1985). Hauptsache eine Lehrstelle: Jugendliche vor den Hürden des Arbeitsmarkts [The main thing is having an apprenticeship: Young people and the hurdles of the labor market]. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Held, T. (1986). Institutionalization and deinstitutionalization of the life course. Human Development, 29, 157–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmke, A. (1987). Entwicklung lern- und leistungsbezogener Motive und Einstellungen: Ergebnisse aus dem SCHOLASTIK-Projekt [Development of learning- and achievement-related motives and attitudes: Findings from the SCHOLASTIK project]. In Weinert, F. E. & Helmke, A. (Eds.), Entwicklung im Grundschulalter (pp. 59–76). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Helmke, A., & Weinert, F. E. (1997). Bedingungsfaktoren schulischer Leistungen [Conditional factors of school achievement]. In Weinert, F. E. (Ed.), Psychologie des Unterrichts und der Schule (Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Themenbereich D, Serie I: Pädagogische Psychologie) (Vol. 3, pp. 71–176). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Helson, H. (1948). Adaptation level as a basis for a quantitative theory of frames of reference. Psychological Review, 55, 297–313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory.New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Helson, H. (1973). A common model for affectivity and perception: An adaption-level approach. In Berlyne, D. E. & Madsen, K. B. (Eds.), Pleasure, reward, preference (pp. 167–188). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Henemann, H. H., & Schwab, D. P. (1972). Evaluation of research on expectancy theory of employee performance. Psychological Bulletin, 78, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henle, M. (1944). The influence of valence on substitution. Journal of Psychology, 17, 11–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hentsch, A. (1992). Motivationale Aspekte des Malens: Eine Anreizanalyse [Motivational aspects of painting: An analysis of incentives]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Hepper, P. G. (1994). Long-term retention of kinship recognition established during infancy in the domestic dog. Behavioural Processes, 33, 3–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermann, M. G. (1980). Assessing the personalities of Soviet politburo members. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 332–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermans, H. J. M. (1970). A questionnaire measure of achievement motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 54, 353–363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, E. H. (1962). Ethology. In Newcomb, T. M. (Ed.), New directions in psychology (Vol. I). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Hess, W. R. (1954). Das Zwischenhirn: Syndrome, Lokalisationen, Emotionen [The interbrain: Syndromes, localizations, emotions]. Basel, Switzerland: Schwabe.Google Scholar
Hesse, F. W., Spies, K., & Lüer, G. (1983). Einfluß motivationaler Faktoren auf das Problemlöseverhalten im Umgang mit komplexen Problemen [Influence of motivational factors on problem-solving behavior in the context of complex problems]. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 30, 400–424.Google Scholar
Hetzer, H. (1931). Kind und Schaffen: Experimente über konstuktive Betätigung im Kleinkindalter [Children and creative work: Experiments on constructive activity in young childhood]. Jena, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Heyman, D., Dweck, C. S., & Cain, L. (1992). Young children's vulnerability to self-blame and helplessness. Child Development, 63, 391–403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyman, D., Gee, C. L., & Giles, J. W. (2003). Preschool children's reasoning about ability. Child Development, 74, 516–534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyn, S., Schnabel, U., & Röder, P. M. (1997). Von der Options- zur Realitätslogik. Stabilität und Wandel berufsbezogener Wertvorstellungen in der Statuspassage Schule-Beruf [Stability and change of career-related values at the school-to-work transition]. In Meier, A., Rabe-Kleberg, U. & Rodax, K. (Eds.), Jahrbuch Bildung und Arbeit ‘97: Transformation und Tradition in Ost und West (S. 281–305). Opladen: Leske and Budrich.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyns, R. W., Veroff, J., & Atkinson, J. W. (1958). A scoring manual for the affiliation motive. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 205–218). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Hidi, S. (2000). An interest researcher's perspective: The effects of extrinsic and intrinsic factors on motivation. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 311–339). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hiemisch, A., Ehlers, A., & Westermann, R. (2002). Mindsets in social anxiety: A new look at selective information processing. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33, 103–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, E. T., & Silberman, I. (1998). Development of regulatory focus: Promotion and prevention as ways of living. In Heckhausen, J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Motivation and self regulation across the life span (pp. 78–113). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, R. L., Snyder, C. R., & Berglas, S. (Eds.). (1990). Self-handicapping: The paradox that isn't. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hillgruber, A. (1912). Fortlaufende Arbeit und Willensbetätigung [Continuous work and use of volition]. Untersuchungen zur Psychologie und Philosophie, 1, 6.Google Scholar
Himmi, T., Boyer, A., & Orsini, J. C. (1988). Changes in lateral hypothalamic neuronal activity accompanying hyper- and hypoglycemias. Physiology & Behavior, 44, 347–354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinde, R. A. (1974). The study of aggression: Determinants, consequences, goals, and functions. In Wit, J. & Hartup, W. W. (Eds.), Determinants and origins of aggressive behavior (pp. 3–27). The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton.Google Scholar
Hiroto, D. W., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Generality of learned helplessness in man. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 311–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodoka, A., & Fincham, F. D. (1995). Origins of children's helpless and mastery achievement patterns in the family. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 375–385.Google Scholar
Hofer, J., & Chasiotis, A. (2004). Methodological considerations of applying a TAT-type picture-story test in cross-cultural research: A comparison of German and Zambian adolescents. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 224–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis: Die Funktion von Antizipationen in der menschlichen Verhaltenssteuerung und Wahrnehmung [Prediction and insight: The function of anticipation in human behavioral control and perception]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Hoffmann, N. (1998). Zwänge und Depressionen: Pierre Janet und die Verhaltenstherapie [Compulsions and depressions: Pierre Janet and behavioral therapy]. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogan, J., & Hogan, J. (1995). Hogan Personality Inventory Manual. Tulsa, OK: Hogan Assessment Systems.Google Scholar
Hogan, J., & Ones, D. S. (1997). Conscientiousness and integrity at work. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 849–870). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hogan, R. (1996). A socioanalytic perspective on the five-factor model. In Wiggins, J. S. (Ed.), The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 163–179). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hogan, R. (1981). Transitions and the life course. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hokoda, A., & Fincham, F. D. (1995). Origins of children's helpless and mastery achievement patterns in the family. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 375–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holder, W. B., Marx, M. N., Holder, E. E., & Collier, G. (1957). Response strength as a function of delay in a runway. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 316–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holland, J. L. (1997). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work environments. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Holowka, S., & Petitto, L. A. (2002). Left hemisphere cerebral specialization for babies with babbling. Science, 297, 1515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoppe, F. (1930). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs- und Affektpsychologie. IX. Erfolg und Misserfolg [Studies of the psychology of action and affect. IX. Success and failure]. Psychologische Forschung, 14, 1–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hörhold, M., & Walschburger, P. (1997). Depressive Störung als Ausdruck misslingender Handlungskontrolle: Überprüfung einer psychophysiologischen Belastungsdiagnostik [Depressive disorders as an expression of failed action control: Testing a psychophysiological assessment]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie: Forschung und Praxis, 26, 31–27.Google Scholar
Hormuth, S. E. (1986). The sampling of experience in situ. Journal of Personality, 54, 262–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horner, M. S. (1974a). Performance of men in noncompetitive and interpersonal competitive achievement-oriented situations. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 237–254). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Horner, M. S. (1974b). The measurement and behavioral implications of fear of succes in women. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 91–117). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Hosenfeld, B. (1988). Persönlichkeitsveränderungen im Erwachsenenalter aus der Sicht Jugendlicher [Personality changes in adulthood from the perspective of adolescents]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Free University of Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Housten, D. M. (1995). Vulnerability to depressive mood reactions: Retesting the hoplessness model of depression. British Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 293–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hovland, C. I., & Sears, R. R. (1938). Experiments on motor conflict: I. Types of conflict and their modes of resolution. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 477–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudley, C., & Graham, S. (1993). An attributional intervention to reduce peer directed aggression among African-American boys. Child Development, 64, 124–138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hull, C. L. (1930). Knowledge and purpose as habit mechanisms. Psychological Review, 37, 511–525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1932). The goal and maze learning. Psychological Review, 39, 25–434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1933). Differential habituation to internal stimuli in the albino rat. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 16, 255–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1934). The concept of the habit-family hierarchy, and maze learning. Psychological Review, 41, 33–54; 134–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1952). A behavior system: An introduction to behavior theory concerning the individual organism. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Hundertmark, J. (1990). Entwicklungsbezogene Intentionen im Lebenslauf: Selbstbild und normative Entwicklungsvorstellungen als Einflußfaktoren [Development-related intentions across the lifespan: The role of self-related and normative conceptions]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, Technical University of Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Hundertmark, J., & Heckhausen, J. (1994). Entwicklungsziele junger, mittelalter und alter Erwachsener [Developmental goals in young, middle, and old adulthood]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 26, 197–217.Google Scholar
Hunt, J. M. V. (1965). Intrinsic motivation and its role in psychological development. In Levine, D. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 13 (pp. 189–282). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Hyland, M. E., Curtis, C., & Mason, D. (1985). Fear of success: Motive and cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1669–1677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ikemoto, S., & Panksepp, J. (1999). The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: A unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Research Reviews, 31(1), 6–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T. R. (1997). A neurobiological basis of social attachment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(6), 726–735.Google ScholarPubMed
Insel, T. R., Winslow, J. T., Wang, Z., & Young, L. J. (1998). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neuroendocrine basis of pair bond formation. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, 449, 215–224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irani, B. G., & Haskell-Luevano, C. (2005). Feeding effects of melanocortin ligands – A historical perspective. Peptides, 26(10), 1788–1799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irle, M., & Krolage, J. (1973). Kognitive Konsequenzen irrtümlicher Selbsteinschätzung [Cognitive implications of erroneous self-evaluations]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 4, 36–50.Google Scholar
Irwin, F. W. (1953). Stated expectations as functions of probability and desirability of outcomes. Journal of Personality, 21, 329–339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irwin, F. W. (1971). Intentional behavior and motivation: A cognitive theory. Philadelphia: Lippincott.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion.New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, D. N. (1974). Manual for the Personality Research Form. Goshen, NY: Research Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Jackson, L. A., & Larrance, D. T. (1979). Is a “refinement” of attribution theory necessary to accommodate the learned helplessness reformulation? A critique of the reformulation of Abramson, Seligman & Teasdale. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 681–682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: The flow state scale 2 and dispositional flow scale 2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24, 133–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, B. (1958). A method for investigating the cue characteristics of pictures. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 617–629). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Jagacinski, C. M., & Nicholls, J. G. (1987). Competence and affect in task involvement and ego involvement: The impact of social comparison information. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 107–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jagacinski, C. M., & Nicholls, J. G. (1990). Reducing effort to protect perceived ability: They'd do it but I wouldn't. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 15–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (2 vols).New York: Holt.Google Scholar
James, W. (1892). Psychology: The briefer course. New York: Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janke, W., Erdmann, G., & Kallus, W. (1985). Stressverarbeitungsfragebogen (SVF) [Stress management questionnaire (SVF)]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Janos, O., & Papoušek, H. (1977). Acquisition of appetition and palpebral conditioned reflexes by the same infants. Early Human Development, 1, 91–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaudas, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2004, April). Führen Vorsätze zu Rigidität im Zielstreben? [Do implementation intentions lead to rigidity in goal pursuit?] Paper presented at the symposium “Recent developments in research on implementation intentions” at the 46th meeting of the Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen (TeaP), Giessen, Germany.Google Scholar
Jemmott, J. B. III (1982). Psychosocial stress, social motives and disease susceptibility. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Jemmott, J. B. (1987). Social motives and susceptibility to disease: Stalking individual differences in health risks. Journal of Personality, 55, 267–298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jemmott, J. B., Borysenko, J. Z., Borysenko, M., McClelland, D. C., Chapman, R., Meyer, D., & Benson, H. (1983). Academic stress, power motivation, and decrease in salivary secretory immunoglobulin A secretion rate. Lancet, 1400–1402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jemmott, J. B., & Locke, S. E. (1984). Psychosocial factors, immunologic mediation, and human susceptibility to infections diseases: How much do we know?Psychological Bulletin, 95, 78–108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkins, S. R. (1994). Need for achievement and women's careers over 14 years: Evidence for occupational structure effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 922–932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, T. N., Warner, L. H., & Warden, C. J. (1926). Standard apparatus for the study of animal behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 6, 361–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jennings, K. D. (1991). Early development of mastery motivation and its relation to the self-concept. In Bullock, M. (Ed.), The development of intentional action: Cognitive, motivational and interactive processes. Contributions to human development (pp. 1–13). Basel, Switzerland: Karger.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E. (1976). How do people perceive the causes of behavior?American Scientist, 64, 237–246.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 219–266). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E., & McGillis, D. (1976). Correspondent inferences and the attribution cube: A comparative appraisal. In Harvey, J. H., Ickes, W. J., & Kidd, R. F. (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (Vol. 1, pp. 387–420). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. New York: General Learning.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E., Worchel, S., Goethals, G. R., & Grumet, J. F. (1971). Prior expectancy and behavioral extremity as determinants of attitude attribution. Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 7, 59–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, E. E., Rock, L., Shaver, K. G., Goethals, G. R., & Ward, L. M. (1968). Pattern of performance and ability attribution: An unexpected primacy effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 317–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, H. E., & Jones, M. C. (1928). A study of fear. Childhood Education, 5, 136–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, S. C. (1973). Self- and interpersonal evaluations: Esteem theories versus consistency theories. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 185–199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jopt, U.-J. (1974). Extrinsische Motivation und Leistungsverhalten [Extrinsic motivation and achievement behavior]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Joule, R. V., & Beauvois, J.-L. (1998). Cognitive dissonance theory: A radical view. European Review of Social Psychology, 8, 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jucknat, M. (1938). Leistung, Anspruchsniveau und Selbstbewußtsein [Achievement, level of aspiration, and self-confidence]. Psychologische Forschung, 22, 89–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jung, C. G. (1936/1990). Typologie [Typology]. Munich, Germany: dtv.Google Scholar
Jungermann, H., Pfister, H.-R., & May, R. S. (1994). Competing motivations or changing choices: Conjectures and some data on choice-action consistency. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 195–208). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Junker, E. (1960). Über unterschiedliches Behalten eigener Leistungen[On the differing retention of one's own achievement]. Frankfurt, Germany: Waldemar Kramer.Google Scholar
Kafka, F. (1986). Briefe an Milena [Letters to Milena]. Frankfurt am Main: Fischer.Google Scholar
Kästele, G. (1988). Anlage- und umweltbedingte Determinanten der Handlungs- und Lageorientierung nach Misserfolg im Vergleich zu anderen Persönlichkeitseigenschaften [Genetic and environmental determinants of failure-related action and state orientation in compari son with other personality traits]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Osnabrück, Germany.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1988). The meanings of personality predicates. American Psychologist, 43, 614–620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, S. (1981). Ecology and the acculturation of cognitive and social styles among Mexican American children. Hispanic Journal of Behavior Sciences, 3(2), 111–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). On the interpretation of intuitive probability: A reply to Jonathan Cohen. Cognition, 7, 409–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39, 341–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kammer, D. (1983). Eine Untersuchung der psychometrischen Eigenschaften des deutschen Beck-Depressionsinventars (BDI) [An investigation of the psychometric properties of the German Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)]. Diagnostica, 29, 48–60.Google Scholar
Kammer, D., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1987). Depressive Attribuierung: Eine Standortbestimmung [Depressive attribution: The state of the art]. In Ameland, M. (Ed.), Bericht über den 35. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie in Heidelberg (Vol. 2, pp. 557–566). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kanfer, R. (1990). Motivation theory and industrial and organizational psychology. In Dunnette, M. D. & Hough, L. M. (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 75–170). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Karabenick, J. D., & Heller, K. A. (1976). A developmental study of effort and ability attributions. Developmental Psychology, 12, 559–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karabenick, S. A. (1972). Valence of success and failure as a function of achievement motives and locus of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 101–110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karabenick, S. A., & Yousseff, Z. I. (1968). Performance as a function of achievement level and perceived difficulty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 414–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karasawa, M., Little, T. D., Miyashita, T., Mashima, M., & Azuma, H. (1997). Japanese children's action-control beliefs about school performance. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 405–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karau, S. J., & Williams, K. D. (1993). Social loafing: A meta-analytic review and theoretical integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 681–706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufmann, I. C., & Rosenblum, L. A. (1969). The reaction of separation from mother on the emotional behavior of infant monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 159, 681–695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K. (1977a). Thickening thin data: The maternal role in developing communication and language. In Bullowa, M. (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 191–206). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. (1977b). Toward the origin of dialogue. In Schaffer, H. R. (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp. 89–117). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kazén, M., Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Self-infiltration vs. self-compatibility checking in dealing with unattractive tasks and unpleasant items: The moderating influence of state vs. action orientation. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 157–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kazén, M., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Intention memory and achievement motivation: Volitional facilitation and inhibition as a function of affective contents of need-related stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 426–448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keenan, J. P., Nelson, A., Connor, O' M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Self-recognition and the right hemisphere. Nature, 409, 305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keeney, R. L., & Reiffa, H. (1976). Decisions with multiple objectives: Preferences and value tradeoffs. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kehr, H. M. (2004a). Implicit(explicit motive discrepancies and volitional depletion among managers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 315–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehr, H. M. (2004b). Integrating implicit motives, explicit motives, and perceived abilities: The compensatory model of work motivation and volition. Academy of Management Review, 29, 479–499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H. (1997a). Entwicklungspsychopathologie: Das Entstehen von Verhaltensproblemen in der frühesten Kindheit. [Develop mental psychopathology: The emergence of behavioral problems in infancy]. In Keller, H. (Ed.), Handbuch der Kleinkindforschung (pp. 625–641). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Keller, H. (1997b). Kontinuität und Entwicklung [Continuity and development]. In Keller, H. (Ed.), Handbuch der Kleinkindforschung. (pp. 235–258). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Keller, H. (1997c). Evolutionary approaches. In Berry, J., Poortinga, Y., & Panndey, J. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, theory and method (Vol. 1, pp. 215–255). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Keller, H. (2000). Human parent-child relationships from an evolutionary perspective. American Behavioral Scientist, 43, 957–969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., & Gauda, G. (1987). Eye contact in the first months of life and its developmental consequences. In Rauh, H. & Steinhausen, H.-C. (Eds.), Psychobiology and early development (advances in psychology) (pp. 129–143). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Keller, H., Lohaus, A., Völker, S., Cappenberg, M., & Chasiotis, A. (1999). Temporal contingency as a measure of interactional quality. Child Development, 70, 474–485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., Lohaus, A., Völker, S., Elben, C., & Ball, J. (2003). Warmth and contingency and their relationship to maternal attitudes toward parenting. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164, 275–292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In Levine, D. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 192–238). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Kelley, H. H. (1971). Attribution in social interaction. New York: General Learning.Google Scholar
Kelley, H. H. (1972). Causal schemata and the attribution process. New York: General Learning.Google Scholar
Kelley, H. H. (1973). The process of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, H. H., & Michela, J. L. (1980). Attribution theory and research. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 457–501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, G. (1962). Comments on J. Brehm “Motivational effects of cognitive dissonance.” In Jones, M. P. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 78–81). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keltner, D., & Robinson, R. J. (1996). Extremism, power, and the imagined basis of social conflict. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 101–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keltner, D., & Robinson, R. J. (1997). Defending the status quo: Power and bias in social conflict. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1066–1077.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemmler, L. (1957). Untersuchungen über den frühkindlichen Trotz [Studies on defiance in early childhood]. Psychologische Forschung, 25, 279–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenrick, D. T., Li, N. P., & Butner, J. (2003). Dynamical evolutionary psychology: Individual decision rules and emergent social norms. Psychological Review, 110, 3–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendrick, K. M. (2004). The neurobiology of social bonds. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 16(12), 1007–1008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keverne, E. B., & Curley, J. P. (2004). Vasopressin, oxytocin and social behaviour. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(6), 777–783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keverne, E. B., & Kendrick, K. M. (1994). Maternal behaviour in sheep and its neuroendocrine regulation. Acta Paediatrica, 397(Suppl), 47–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keverne, E. B., Martensz, N. D., & Tuite, B. (1989). Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 14(1–2), 155–161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killcross, S., Robbins, T. W., & Everitt, B. J. (1997). Different types of fear-conditioned behaviour mediated by separate nuclei within amygdala. Nature, 388(6640), 377–380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, J. E., & Figueredo, A. J. (1997). The five-factor model plus dominance in chimpanzee personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 257–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, L. A. (1995). Wishes, motives, goals, and personal memories: Relations of measures of human motivation. Journal of Personality, 63, 985–1007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kipnis, D. (1972). Does power corrupt?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 33–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kipnis, D. (1974). The powerholder. In Tedeschi, J. T. (Ed.), Perspectives on social power (pp. 82–122). Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
Kipnis, D. (1976). The powerholders. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kircher, T. T. J., Brammer, M., Bullmore, E., Simmons, A., Bartels, M., & David, A. S. (2002). The neural correlates of intentional and incidental self processing. Neuropsychologia, 40, 683–692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirschbaum, C., Wolf, O., Wippich, W., & Hellhammer, D. (1996). Stress- and treatment-induced elevations of cortisol levels associated with impaired declarative memory in healthy adults. Life Science, 58, 1475–1483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klandermans, P. G. (1983). Rotter's I-E-scale and socio-political action-taking: The balance of 20 years of research. European Journal of Social Psychology, 13, 399–415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, S. B., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Chance, S. (2002). Decisions and the evolution of memory: Multiple systems, multiple functions. Psychological Review, 109, 306–329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinbeck, U. (1975). Motivation und Berufswahl [Motivation and occupational choice]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kleinbeck, U. (1996). Arbeitsmotivation: Entstehung, Wirkung und Förderung [Work motivation: Origins, effects, and promotion]. Weinheim, Germany: Juventa.Google Scholar
Klineberg, D. (1938). Emotional expression in Chinese literature. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 33, 517–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klinger, E. (1967). Modelling effects on achievement imagery. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 49–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klinger, E. (1971). Structure and functions of fantasy. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review, 82, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and the incentives in people's lives. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Klüver, H., & Bucy, P. C. (1937). “Psychic blindness” and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobectomy in rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Physiology, 119, 352–353.Google Scholar
Klüver, H., & Bucy, P. C. (1939). Preliminary analysis of functions of the temporal lobes in monkeys. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 42, 979–1000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koch, S. (1956). Behavior as “intrinsically” regulated: Work notes towards a pre-theory of phenomena called “motivational.” In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 42–87). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Koch, S. (Ed.) (1959–1963). Psychology: A study of science. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 1091–1111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kock, S. E. (1965). Företagsledning och motivation. Svenska Handeinshögskolan, Affärsekonomiska Förlagsförenigen, Helsingfors.Google Scholar
Kock, S. E. (1974). Företagsledning och motivation. Nordisk Psykologi, 26, 211–219.Google Scholar
Koepp, M. J., Gunn, R. N., Lawrence, A. D., Cunningham, V. J., Dagher, A., Jones, T., Brooks, D. J., Bench, C. J., & Grasby, P. M. (1998). Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature, 393(6682), 266–268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koestner, R., & McClelland, D. C. (1990). Perspectives on competence motivation. In Pervin, L. (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 527–548). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Koestner, R., Weinberger, J., & McClelland, D. C. (1991). Task-intrinsic and social-extrinsic sources of arousal for motives assessed in fantasy and self-report. Journal of Personality, 59, 57–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koestner, R., Zuckerman, M., & Koestner, J. (1987). Praise, involvement and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(2), 383–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohli, M. (1981). Zur Theorie der biographischen Selbst- und Fremdthematisierung [Theory of biographical accounts of the self and others]. In Matthes, J. (Ed.), Lebenswelt und soziale Probleme: Verhandlungen des 20. Deutschen Soziologentages (pp. 502–520). Frankfurt, Germany: Campus.Google Scholar
Kohut, H. (1979). Die Heilung des Selbst [Healing the self]. Frankfurt, Germany: Suhrkamp.Google Scholar
Köller, O. (2000). Leistungsgruppierung, soziale Vergleiche und selbstbezogene Fähigkeitskognitionen in der Schule [Ability grouping, social comparisons, and self-related ability cognitions in school]. Habilitationsschrift, Univeristy of Potsdam, Germany.Google Scholar
Köller, O., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., & Baumert, J. (2006). Zum Zusammenspiel von schulischer Leistung, Selbstkonzept und Interesse in der gymnasialen Oberstufe [On the interplay of academic achievement, self-concept, and interest in upper secondary schools]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 20, 27–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konner, M. J. (1981). Evolution of human behavioral development. In Monroe, R. H., Monroe, R. L., & Whiting, B. B. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural human development (pp. 3–51). New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Koole, S. L. (2000). Positivity in self-evaluation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Free University of Amsterdam, Netherlands.Google Scholar
Koole, S. L. (2004). Volitional shielding of the self: Effects of action orientation and external demand on implicit self-evaluation. Social Cognition, 22, 117–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koole, S. L., & Jostmann, N. (2004). Getting a grip on your feelings: Effects of action orientation and social demand on intuitive affect regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(6), 974–989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koole, S. L., Smeets, K., Knippenberg, A., & Dijksterhuis, A. (1999). The cessation of rumination through self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 111–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kornadt, H.-J. (1982a). Band 1: Empirische u. theoretische Untersuchungen zu einer Motivationstheorie der Aggression und zur Konstruktvalidierung eines Aggressions-TAT [Volume 1: Empirical and theoretical studies on a motivational theory of aggression and on the construct validation of an aggression TAT]. Aggressionsmotiv und Aggressionshemmung.Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Kornadt, H.-J. (1982b). Grundzüge einer Motivationstheorie der Aggression [Outline of a motivational theory of aggression]. In Hilke, R. & Kempf, W. (Eds.), Aggression (pp. 86–111). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Kornadt, H.-J., Eckensberger, L. H., & Emminghaus, W. B. (1980). Cross-cultural research on motivation and its contribution to a general theory of motivation. In Triandis, H. C. (Ed.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology (Vol. 3: Basic processes) (pp. 223–321). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435(7042), 673–676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krampen, G. (1987). Differential effects of teacher comments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 137–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krantz, D. L., & Allen, D. (1967). The rise and fall of McDougall's instinct doctrine. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 3, 326–338.3.0.CO;2-6>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krapp, A. (1999). Intrinsische Lernmotivation und Interesse: Forschungsansätze und konzeptuelle Überlegungen [Intrinsic learning motivation and interest: Research approaches and conceptual considerations]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 45, 387–406.Google Scholar
Krapp, A. (2001). Interesse [Interest]. In Rost, D. H. (Ed.), Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie (2nd ed., pp. 286–293). Weinheim, Germany: PVU.Google Scholar
Krapp, A., Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. (1992). Interest, learning and development. In Renninger, K. A. & Hidi, S. (Eds.), The role of interest and development(pp. 3–25). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Krau, E. (1982). Motivational feedback loops in the structure of action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1030–1040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, J. R. (1980). Optimal foraging, predation risk and territory defense. Area, 68, 83–90.Google Scholar
Krech, D., Crutchfield, R. S., & Ballachey, E. L. (1962). Individual in society.New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Krohne, H. W. (1988). Erziehungsstil forschung: Neuere theoretische Ansätze und empirische Befunde [Child-rearing research: New theoretical approaches and empirical results]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 2, 157–172.Google Scholar
Krohne, H. W. (1996). Angst und Angstbewältigung [Anxiety and coping with anxiety]. Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Krug, S. (1976). Förderung und Änderung des Leistungsmotivs: Theoretische Grundlagen und deren Anwendung [Fostering and modifying the achievement motive: Theoretical fundamentals and their application]. In Schmalt, H.-D. & Meyer, W.-U. (Eds.), Leistungsmotivation und Verhalten (pp. 221–247). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett.Google Scholar
Krüger, H. (1978). Anfänge der Entwicklung des Anstrengungskonzepts im Kindergartenalter [Early development of the effort concept at kindergarten age]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Krüger, J., & Heckhausen, J. (1993). Personality development across the adult life span: Subjective conceptions versus cross-sectional contrasts. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 48, 100–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krüger, J., Heckhausen, J., & Hundertmark, J. (1995). Perceiving middle aged adults: Effects of stereotype-congruent and incongruent information. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 50B, 82–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cognitive and motivational bases. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.). (1996). Social psychology: A handbook of basic principles (pp. 329–360). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kubinger, K. D., & Ebenhöh, H. (1996). Arbeitshaltungen (AHA): Objektiver Persönlichkeitstest [Work styles: Objective personality test]. Mödling, Austria: Schuhfried.Google Scholar
Kubinger, K. D., & Litzenberger, M. (2003). Zur Validität der Objektiven Persönlichkeits-Test-Batterie “Arbeitshaltungen” [On the validity of the objective personality test battery “Work Style”]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 24, 119–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Külpe, O. (1893). Grundriß der Psychologie. Auf experimenteller Grundlage dargestellt [Outlines of Psychology. Based upon the Results of Experimental Investigation]. Leipzig, Germany: Wilhelm Engelmann.Google Scholar
Kuester, J., & Paul, A. (1989). Reproductive strategies of subadult barbary macaque males at Affenberg Salem. In Rasa, A. E., Vogel, C., & Voland, E. (Eds.), The sociobiology of sexual and reproductive strategies (pp. 93–109). London: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1977). Meß- und prozeßtheoretische Analysen einiger Person- und Situationsparameter der Leistungsmotivation[Personal and situational determinants of achievement motivation: Computer simulation and experimental analysis]. Bonn, Germany: Bouvier.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1978a). Situations-, reaktions- und personbezogene Konsistenz des Leistungsmotivs bei der Messung mittels des Heckhausen TAT [Situation-, reaction-, and person-related consistency of the achievement motive measured using Heckhausen's TAT]. Archiv für Psychologie, 130, 37–52.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1978b). Standard setting and risk preference: An elaboration of the theory of achievement motivation and an empirical test. Psychological Review, 85, 239–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1981). Motivational and functional helplessness: The moderating effect of state vs. action orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 155–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1982). The expectancy-value approach in the theory of social motivation. In Feather, N. T. (Ed.), Expectations and actions: Expectancy-value models in psychology (pp. 125–162). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1983). Motivation, Konflikt und Handlungskontrolle [Motivation, conflict, and action control]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1984). Motivational aspects of achievement motivation and learned helplessness: Toward a comprehensive theory of action control. In Maher, B. A. & Maher, W. B. (Eds.), Progress in experimental personality research (Vol. 13, pp. 99–171). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1987). Action control: The maintenance of motivational states. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 279–291). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1994). Action versus state orientation: Psychometric properties of the Action-Control-Scale (ACS-90). In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Action control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 47–59). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1996). Wille und Freiheitserleben: Formen der Selbststeuerung [The will and perceived freedom: Forms of volition]. In Kuhl, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Motivation, Volition und Handlung(Series IV, Vol. 4, pp. 665–765). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (1998). Wille und Persönlichkeit: Von der Funktionsanalyse zur Aktivierungsdynamik psychischer Systeme [Volition and personality: Functional analysis of self-regulation]. Psychologische Rundschau, 49, 61–77.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (2000a). A functional-design approach to motivation and volition: The dynamics of personality systems interactions. In Boekaerts, M., Pintrich, P. R., & Zeidner, M. (Eds.), Self-regulation: Directions and challenges for future research (pp. 111–169). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (2000b). A theory of self-development: Affective fixation and the STAR model of personality disorders and related styles. In Heckhausen, J. (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 187–211). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J. (2000c). The volitional basis of personality systems interaction theory: Applications in learning and treatment contexts. International Journal of Educational Research, 33, 665–703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J. (2001). Motivation und Persönlichkeit: Die Interaktion psychischer Systeme [Motivation and personality: The interaction of psychological systems]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Baumann, N. (2000). Self-regulation and rumination: Negative affect and impaired self-accessibility. In Perrig, W. & Grob, A. (Eds.), Control of human behavior mental processes and consciousness: Essays in honor of the 60th birthday of August Flammer (pp. 283–305). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1983). Handlungskontrolle und Umfang der Informa tionsverarbeitung: Wahl einer einfachen (nicht optimalen) Entscheidungsregel zug unsten rascher Handlungsbereitschaft [Action control and scope of information processing: Selection of a simplified (non-optimal) decision rule to achieve fast readiness for action]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 14, 241–250.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1985). Action control theory: From cognition to behavior. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1994a). Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1994b). Alienation: Ignoring one's preferences. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Blankenship, V. (1979). The dynamic theory of achievement motivation: From episodic to dynamic thinking. Psychological Review, 86, 141–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Fuhrmann, A. (1998). Decomposing self-regulation and self-control: The volitional components checklist. In Heckhausen, J. & Dweck, C. (Eds.), Life span perspectives on motivation and control (pp. 15–49). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Goschke, T. (1994). State orientation and the activation and retrieval of intentions from memory. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 127–152). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Helle, P. (1986). Motivational and volitional determinants of depression: The degenerated intention hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 247–251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, J., & Kaschel, R. (2004). Entfremdung als Krankheitsursache: Selbstregulation von Affekten und integrative Kompetenz [Alienation as a determinant of symptom formation: Self-regulation of affect and integrative competence]. Psychologische Rundschau, 55, 61–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (1994). Self-discrimination and memory: State orientation and false ascription of assigned activities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 1103–1115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (1999). Volitional facilitation of difficult intentions: Joint activation of intention memory and positive affect removes Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 382–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (2003). Handlungs- und Lageorientierung: Wie lernt man, seine Gefühle zu steuern? [Action and state orientation: How do people learn to control their feelings?] In Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. & Rheinberg, F. (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (2004). Impress them or convince them? Sales performance, social needs and psychological well-being as a function of histrionic vs. action oriented personality. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Kuhl, J., & Scheffer, D. (1999). Der operante Multi-Motiv-Test (OMT): Manual [The operant Multi-Motive Test (OMT): Manual]. University of Osnabrück, Germany.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Völker, S. (1998). Entwicklung und Persönlichkeit [Development and personality]. In Keller, H. (Ed.), Lehrbuch der Entwicklungspsychologie (pp. 207–240). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Kuhl, J., & Weiß, M. (1994). Performance deficits following uncontrollable failure: Impaired action control or global attributions and generalized expectancy deficits? In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation.Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kukla, A. (1972a). Attributional determinants of achievement-related behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 166–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kukla, A. (1972b). Foundations of an attributional theory of performance. Psychological Review, 79, 454–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kukla, A. (1978). An attributional theory of choice. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 113–144). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kummer, H. (1995). Causal knowledge in animals. In Sperber, D., Premack, D., & Premack, A. James (Eds.), A causal cognition (pp. 26–39). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Kun, A. (1977). Evidence for preschoolers' understanding of causal direction in extended causal sequences. Child Development, 49, 218–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kun, A., Parsons, J. E., & Ruble, D. (1974). The development of integration processes using ability and effort information to predict outcome. Developmental Psychology, 10 (5), 721–732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kun, A., & Weiner, B. (1973). Necessary versus sufficient causal schemata for success and failure. Journal of Research in Personality, 7, 197–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunda, Z. (1999). Social cognition: Making sense of people. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kunde, W., Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation and execution of actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 57A, 87–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lacey, J. I. (1969). Somatic response patterning and stress: Some revisions of activation theory. In Appley, M. H. & Trumbull, R. (Eds.), Psychological stress: Issues and research (pp. 14–39). New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
Lang, F. R. (2001). Regulation of social relationship in later adulthood. Journal of Gerontology, Psychological Sciences, 56B, 321–329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, F. R. (2004). Social motivation across the life span. In Lang, F. R. & Fingerman, K. L. (Eds.), Growing together: Personal relationships across the lifespan (pp. 341–367). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lang, F. R., & Heckhausen, J. (2001). Perceived control over development and subjective well-being: Differential benefits across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 509–523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, F. R., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). Stabilisierung und Kontinuität der Persönlichkeit im Lebensverlauf [Stabilization and continuity of personality across the lifespan]. In Asendorpf, J. B. (Ed.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie (Vol. C/V/3). Soziale, emotionale und Persönlichkietsentwicklung (pp. 525–562). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Lang, F. R., & Heckhausen, J. (2006). Developmental changes of motivation and interpersonal capacities across adulthood: Managing the challenges and constraints of social contexts. In Hoare, C. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of adult development and learning (pp. 149–166). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lang, F. R., Reschke, F. S., & Neyer, F. J. (2006). Social relationships, transitions and personality development across the life span. In Mroczek, D. K. & Little, T. D. (Eds.), Handbook of personality development (pp. 445–466). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. H. (1999). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. Gainesville: NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, University of Florida. (this reference to Lang et al. replaces the one to CEA, please substitute in master reference list)Google Scholar
Lange, L. (1888). Neue Experimente über den Vorgang der einfachen Reaktion auf Sinneseindrücke [New experiments on the process of simple reaction to sense impressions]. Philosophische Studien, 4, 479–510.Google Scholar
Langens, T. A. (2001). Predicting behavior change in Indian businessmen from a combination of need for achievement and self-discrepancy. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 339–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langens, T. A. (2002). Tagträume, Anliegen und Motivation [Daydreams, personal goals, and motivation]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Lansing, J. B., & Heyns, R. W. (1959). Need affiliation and frequency of four types of communication. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 365–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latané, B., Williams, K., & Harkins, S. (1979). Many hands make light the work: The causes and consequences of social loafing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 822–832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawler, E. E. (1968). A correlational-causal analysis of the relationship between expectancy attitudes and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 52, 462–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawler, E. E., & Porter, L. W. (1967). Antecedent attitudes of effective managerial job performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 2, 122–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, D. M., & Festinger, L. (1962). Deterrents and reinforcement: The psychology of insufficient reward. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Lawrence, P. R., & Nohria, N. (2002). Driven: How human nature shapes our choices. San Francisco: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. A., & Launier, R. (1979). Stress-related transactions between person and environment. In Pervin, L. A. & Lewis, M. (Eds.), Perspectives in interactional psychology (pp. 287–372). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1968). Emotion and adaption: Conceptual and empirical relations. In Arnold, W. J. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 175–270). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. American Psychologist, 39, 124–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarus, R. S., Opton, E. M., Nomikos, M. S., & Rankin, N. D. (1965). The principle of short-circuiting of threat: Further evidence. Journal of Personality, 33, 622–635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnen, J., & Tallon, S. (1966). La periodicité spontanée de la prise d'aliments ad libitum du rat blanc. Journal de Physiologie, 58, 323–349.Google Scholar
Lecci, L., Karoly, P., Briggs, C., & Kuhn, K. (1994). Specificity and generality of motivational components in depression: A personal projects analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 404–408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. (1995). Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 209–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Touchstone.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (2002). The synaptic self. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Lee, F. K., Sheldon, K. M., & Turban, D. B. (2003). Personality and the goal-striving process: The influence of achievement goal patterns, goal level and mental focus on performance and enjoyment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 256–265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leeper, R. W. (1935). The role of motivation in learning: A study of the phenomenon of differential motivational control of the utilization of habits. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 4b, 3–40.Google Scholar
Lefcourt, H. M. (1976). Locus of control: Current trends in theory and research.New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lehmann, H. C., & Witty, P. A. (1934). Faculty psychology and personality traits. American Journal of Psychology, 46, 486–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leibowitz, S. F., Weiss, G. F., Walsh, U. A., & Viswanath, D. (1989). Medial hypothalamic serotonin: Role in circadian patterns of feeding and macronutritient selection. Brain Research, 503, 132–140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Reflective and reflexive action control in patients with frontal brain lesions. Neuropsychology, 15, 80–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leon-Villagra, J., Meyer, W. U., & Engler, U. (1990). Ability judgments based on praise: A developmental psychology study. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 22 (1), 54–74.Google Scholar
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the overjustification hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, R. M. (2002). Concepts and theories of human development (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lerner, R. M., & Busch-Rossnagel, N. A. (1981). Individuals as producers of their development: A life-span perspective.New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lersch, P. (1938). Aufbau des Charakters [Structure of the character]. Leipzig, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
Lersch, P. (1951). Aufbau der Person [Structure of the person] (4th ed. of Aufbau des Charakters). Munich, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
LeVay, S., & Hamer, D. H. (1994). Evidence for a biological influence in male homosexuality. Scientific American, May, 44–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leventahl, H. (1984). A perceptual-motor theory of emotion. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 17, pp. 117–182). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Levesque, J., Fanny, E., Joanett, Y., Paquette, V., Mensour, B., Beaudouin, G., Leroux, J.-M., Borugouin, P., & Beauregard, M. (2003). Neural circuitry underlying voluntary suppression of sadness. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 502–510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, A. S., & Billington, C. J. (1997). Why do we eat? A neural systems approach. Annual Review of Nutrition, 17, 597–619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, A. S., & Billington, C. J. (2004). Opioids as agents of reward-related feeding: A consideration of the evidence. Physiology & Behavior, 82 (1), 57–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, A. S., Kotz, C. M., & Gosnell, B. A. (2003). Sugars and fats: The neurobiology of preference. Journal of Nutrition, 133 (3), 831S–834S.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1922). Das Problem der Willensmessung und das Grundgesetz der Assoziation. II [The problem of measuring volition and the law of association. II]. Psychologische Forschung, 2, 65–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1926a). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs- und Affekt-Psychologie I.: Vorbemerkungen über die psychischen Kräfte und Energien und über die Struktur der Seele [Studies of the psychology of action and affect. I: Preliminary remarks on the psychic powers and energies and the structure of the soul]. Psychologische Forschung, 7, 294–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1926b). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs- und Affekt-Psychologie II.: Vorsatz, Wille und Bedürfnis [Studies of the psychology of action and affect. II: Intent, volition, and need]. Psychologische Forschung, 7, 330–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1931a). Die psychologische Situation bei Lohn und Strafe [The psychological elements in rewards and punishment]. Leipzig, Germany: Hirzel.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1931b). Environmental forces in child behavior and development. In Murchison, C. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 94–127). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1932). Ersatzhandlung und Ersatzbefriedigung [Substitute action and substitute satisfaction]. Bericht über den 12. Kongreß der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie, Hamburg, 1931 (pp. 382–384). Jena, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1934). Der Richtungsbegriff in der Psychologie: Der spezielle und allgemeine hodologische Raum [The concept of direction in psychology: Special and general hodological space]. Psychologische Forschung, 19, 249–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality: Selected papers. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of topological psychology.New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1938). The conceptual representation and the measurement of psychological forces. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1939). Field theory and experiment in social psychology. American Journal of Sociology, 44, 868–897.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1942). Field theory of learning. Yearbook of National Social Studies of Education, 41, 215–242.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1943). Defining the “field at a given time.”Psychological Review, 50, 292–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1946a). Action research and minority problems. Journal of Social Issues, 2, 34–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1946b). Behavior and development as a function of the total situation. In Carmichael, L. (Ed.), Manual of child psychology (pp. 791–844). New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1947). Group decision and social change. In Maccoby, E. E., Newcomb, T. M., & Hartley, E. L. (Eds.), Readings in social psychology (pp. 197–211). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1963). Feldtheorie in den Sozialwissenschaften [Field theory in the social sciences]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Lewin, K., Dembo, T., Festinger, L., & Sears, P. S. (1944). Level of aspiration. In McHunt, J. (Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders (Vol. 1, pp. 333–378). New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Toward a theory of social cognition: The development of the self. In Social interaction and communication during infancy: New directions for child developmen (pp. 1–20). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., & Goldberg, S. (1969). Perceptual-cognitive development in infancy: A generalized expectancy model as a function of the mother-infant interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 15, 81–100.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2002). Cortisol response to embarrassment and shame. Child Development, 73, 1034–1045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, N. P., Bailey, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., & Linsenmeier, J. A. W. (2002). The necessities and luxuries of mate preference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 947–955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 529–566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, M. D. (2003). Reflective and reflexive judgment processes: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. In Forgas, J. P., Williams, K. R., & Hippel, W. V. (Eds.), Social judgments: Implicit and explicit processes (pp. 44–67). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Liebert, R. M., & Morris, L. W. (1967). Cognitive and emotional components of text anxiety: A distinction and some initial data. Psychological Reports, 20, 975–978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lienert, G. (1969). Testaufbau und Testanalyse [Test design and test analysis]. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Lindsley, D. B. (1957). Psychophysiology and motivation. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1957 (pp. 44–105). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Lindworsky, J. (1923). Der Wille: Seine Erscheinung und seine Beherrschung [The will: Its emergence and its mastery] (3rd ed.). Leipzig, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2003). The role of self-efficacy beliefs in student engagement and learning in the classroom. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 19, 119–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linville, P. W. (1987). Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer against stress-related illness and depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 663–676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lippitt, R. (1940). An experimental study of the effect of democratic and authoritarian group atmospheres. University of Iowa Studies in Child Welfare, 16, 45–195.Google Scholar
Lissner, K. (1933). Die Entspannung von Bedürfnissen durch Ersatzhandlungen [Relieving needs by substitute actions]. Psychologische Forschung, 18, 218–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, B. R. (1983). Personal projects: A rationale and method for investigation. Environment and Behavior, 15, 273–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, B. R. (1989). Personal projects analysis: Trivial pursuits, magnificent obsessions, and the search for coherence. In Buss, D. M. & Cantor, N. (Eds.), Personality psychology: Recent trends and emerging directions (pp. 15–31). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, B. R. (1999). Personal projects and social ecology: Themes and variations across the life span. In Brandstädter, J. & Lerner, R. (Eds.), Action and self-development: Theory and research through the life span (pp. 197–221). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Little, T. D. (1988). Sociocultural influences on the development of children's action-control beliefs. In Heckhausen, J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulation across the life span (pp. 281–315). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Little, T. D. (1998). Self-regulation and school performance: Is there optimal level of action-control?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70 (1), 54–74.Google Scholar
Little, T. D., & Lopez, D. (1997). Regularities in the development of children's causality beliefs about school performance across six sociocultural contexts. Developmental Psychology, 33, 165–175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, T. D., Lopez, D., Oettingen, G., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). A comparative-longitudinal study of action-control beliefs and school performance: On the role of context. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 237–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, T. D., Oettingen, G., Stetsenko, A., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Children's action-control beliefs about school performance: How do American children compare with German and Russian children?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 686–700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, T. D., Stetsenko, A., & Maier, H. (1999). Action control beliefs and school performance: A longitudinal study of Moscow children and adolescents. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 799–823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Litwin, G. H. (1966). Achievement motivation, expectancy of success, and risk-taking behavior. In Atkinson, J. W. & Feather, N. T. (Eds.), A theory of achievement behavior (pp. 103–115). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Litwin, G. H., & Stringer, R. A. (1968). Motivation and organizational climate. Boston: Harvard University, Graduate School of Business Administration, Division of Research.Google Scholar
Locke, E. A. (1968). Toward a theory of task motivation and incentives. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 157–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, E. A. (1975). Personnel attitudes and motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 26, 457–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation – A 35 year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 705–717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Locke, E. A., & Shaw, K. N. (1984). Atkinson's inverse-U curve and the missing cognitive variables. Psychological Reports, 55, 403–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loehlin, J. C. (1989). Partitioning environmental and genetic contributions to behavioral development. American Psychologist, 44, 1285–1292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, G. D., Schachar, R. J., & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological Science, 8, 60–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez, D. F., Little, T. D., Oettingen, G., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Self regulation and school performance: Is there optimal level of action-control?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 54–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorberbaum, J. P., Newman, J. D., Horwitz, A. R., Dubno, J. R., Lydiard, R. B., Hammer, M. B., Bohning, D. E., & George, M. S. (2002). A potential role for thalamocingulate circuitry in human maternal behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 431–445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenz, K. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. Journal für Ornithologie, 83, 137–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1937). Über die Bildung des Instinktbegriffs [On the construction of the concept of instinct]. Naturwissenschaften, 25, 289–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1943). Die angebotenen Formen möglicher Erfahrung [The forms of potential experience]. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 5, 235–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1950). The comparative method of studying innate behavior patterns. In Society for Experimental Biology (Ed.), Physiological mechanisms in animal behavior (pp. 221–268). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1966). Ethologie, die Biologie des Verhaltens [Ethology, the biology of behavior]. In Gessner, F. & Bertalanffy, L. V. (Eds.), Handbuch der Biologie (Vol. II, pp. 341–559). Frankfurt, Germany: Athenäum.Google Scholar
Lovejoy, C. D. (1981). The origin of man. Science, 211, 341–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowell, E. L. (1950). A methodological study of projectively measured achievement motivation. Unpublished master's thesis, Wesleyan University, Middletown, CT.Google Scholar
Lowell, E. L. (1952). The effect of need for achievement on learning and speed of performance. Journal of Psychology, 33, 31–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowin, A. (1967). Approach and avoidance: Alternate modes of selective exposure to information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luginbuhl, J. E. R., Crowe, D. H., & Kahan, J. P. (1975). Causal attribution for success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 86–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luhmann, N. (1975). Macht [Power]. Stuttgart, Germany: Enke.Google Scholar
Lund, B., Rheinberg, F., & Gladasch, U. (2001). Ein Elterntraining zum motivationsförderlichen Erziehungsverhalten in Leistungskontexten [Teaching parents how to reduce children's fear of failure]. Zeits chrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 15, 130–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lundy, A. (1985). The reliability of the Thematic Apperception Test. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 141–145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundy, A. (1988). Instructional set and Thematic Apperception Test validity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 309–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain: An introduction to neuropsychology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R., & Homskaya, E. D. (1964). Disturbances in the regulative role of speech with frontal lobe lesions. In Warren, J. M. & Akert, K. (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior (pp. 353–371). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Lütkenhaus, P. (1984). Pleasure derived from mastery in three-year olds: Its function for persistence and the influence of maternal behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 7, 343–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lütkenhaus, P., & Bullock, M. (1991). Die Entwicklung von funktionalen und deklarativen Aspekten des Selbstkonzeptes bei Kleinkindern [The development of functional and declarative aspects of the self-concept in toddlers]. Paper presented at the 8th Tagung Entwicklungspsychologie, Berne, Switzerland.Google Scholar
Lütkenhaus, P., Grossman, K. E., & Grossman, K. (1985). Transactional influences of infants' orienting ability and maternal cooperation on competition in three-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 80, 257–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, K. (1988). Social and personality development: An evolutionary synthesis. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
MacDonald, K. (1992). Warmth as a developmental construct: An evolutionary analysis. Child Development, 63, 753–773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machiaovelli, N. (1532). Il principe [The prince] (L. Ricci, Trans.). New York: McGraw Hill).Google Scholar
MacIver, D. J., Stipek, D. J., & Daniels, D. H. (1991). Explaining within-semester changes in student effort in junior high school and senior high school courses. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 201–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., & Jetten, J. (1994). Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 808–817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macrae, C. N., Hewstone, M., & Griffiths, R. J. (1993). Processing load and memory for stereotype-based information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 77–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacTurk, R. H., & Morgan, G. A. (1995). Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptualization and applications. Advances in applied developmental psychology (Vol. 12). Westport, CT: Ablex.Google Scholar
Madsen, K. B. (1959). Theories of motivation.Copenhagen, Denmark: Munksgaard.Google Scholar
Madsen, K. B. (1974). Modern theories of motivation. Copenhagen, Denmark: Munksgaard.Google Scholar
Maehr, M. L. (1974). Culture and achievement motivation. American Psychologist, 29, 887–896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maehr, M. L., & Kleiber, D. (1981). The graying of achievement motivation. American Psychologist, 36, 787–793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magnusson, D., & Endler, N. S. (Eds.). (1977). Personality at the crossroads: Current issues in interactional psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mahler, W. (1933). Ersatzhandlungen verschiedenen Realitätsgrades [Substitute actions on various levels of reality]. Psychologische Forschung, 18, 27–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mai, S. (2004). Erfolg im Verkauf – Entwicklung und Evaluation eines kognitiv-behavioralen Trainings [Success in sales – Development and evaluation of a cognitive behavioral training program]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hildesheim, Germany.Google Scholar
Maier, G. W., & Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Action versus state orientation and disengagement from unrealistic goals. Poster presented at the 107th annual convention of the American Psychological Association, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Major, B. (1980). Information acquisition and attribution processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1010–1023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malmo, R. B. (1959). Activation: A neurophysiological dimension. Psychological Review, 66, 367–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Mandler, G., & Sarason, S. B. (1952). A study of anxiety and learning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 166–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mann, P. E., & Bridges, R. S. (2001). Lactogenic hormone regulation of maternal behavior. Progress in Brain Research, 133, 251–262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mansson, H. H. (1969). The relation of dissonance reduction to cognitive, perceptual, consummatory, and learning measures of thirst. In Zimbardo, P. G. (Ed.), The cognitive control of motivation (pp. 78–97). Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
Marbe, K. (1915). Der Begriff der Bewusstseinslage [The concept of mindset]. Fortschritte der Psychologie und ihrer Anwendungen, 3, 27–39.Google Scholar
Marini, M. M. (1984). Age and sequencing norms in the transition to adulthood. Social Forces, 63, 229–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, H. M., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marrow, A. J. (1938). Goal tensions and recall: I. Journal of General Psychology, 19, 3–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W. (1989). Age and sex differences in multiple dimensions of self-concept: Preadolescence to adulthood. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 417–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W., Byrne, B. M., & Shavelson, R. J. (1988). A multifaceted academic self-concept: Its hierarchical structure and its relation to academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 366–380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W., Cairns, L., Relich, J., Barnes, J., & Debus, R. L. (1984). The relationship between dimensions of self-attribution and dimension of self-concept. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 3–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marszal-Wisniewska, M. (2002). Model of volitional and temperamental influences on everyday functioning. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 33, 151–157.Google Scholar
Martin, A. J., Marsh, H. W., & Debus, R. L. (2001). A quadripolar need achievement representation of self-handicapping and defensive pessimism. American Educational Research Journal, 38, 583–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1989). Toward a motivational and structural theory or ruminative thought. In Uleman, J. S. & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 306–326). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In Wyer, R. S. Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 9, pp. 1–47). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Martinez, J. A. (2000). Body-weight regulation: Causes of obesity. Proceedings of the Nutritional Society, 59 (3), 337–345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martire, J. C. (1956). Relationship between the self-concept and differences in the strength and generality of achievement motivation. Journal of Personality, 24, 364–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a psychology of being. New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Mason, A., & Blankenship, V. (1987). Power and affiliation motivation, stress, and abuse in intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 203–210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massimini, F., & Carli, M. (1991). Die systematische Erfassung des Flow-Erlebens im Alltag [The systematic assessment of flow experience in everyday life]. In Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.), Die außergewöhnliche Erfahrung im Alltag: Die Psychologie des Flow-Erlebens (pp. 291–312). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T. (2003). The AI project: Historical and ecological contexts. Animal Cognition, 6 (4), 199–211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, G., & Gilliland, K. (1999). The personality theories of H. J. Eysenck and J. A. Gray: A comparative review. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 583–626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1993). The intelligence of emotional intelligence. Intelligence, 17, 433–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, K. U. (1986). Structural constraints on the life course. Human Development, 29, 163–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, K. U., & Carroll, G. (1987). Jobs and classes: Structural constraints on career mobility. European Sociological Review, 3, 14–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, K. U., & Müller, W. (1986). The state and the structure of the life course. Human development and the life course: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 217–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1974). Behavior programs and evolutionary strategies. American Scientist, 62, 650–659.Google ScholarPubMed
Mazur, A. (1985). A biosocial model of status in face-to-face primate groups. Social Forces, 64, 377–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 353–397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAdams, D. P. (1979). Validation of a thematic coding system for the intimacy motive. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1980). A thematic coding system for the intimacy motive. Journal of Research in Personality, 14, 413–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1982a). Intimacy motivation. In Stewart, A. J. (Ed.), Motivation and society (pp. 133–171). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1982b). Experiences of intimacy and power: Relationship between social motives and autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 292–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P., & Constantian, C. A. (1983). Intimacy and affiliation motives in daily living: An experience sampling analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 851–861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P., Healy, S., & Krause, S. (1984). Social motives and patterns of friendship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 828–838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P., & McClelland., D. C. (1983). Social motives and memory. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University, Department of Psychology, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P., & Powers, J. (1981). Themes of intimacy in behavior and thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 573–587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P., & Vaillant, G. E. (1982). Intimacy motivation and psychosocial adjustment: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 46, 586–593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArthur, L. A. (1972). The how and what of why: Some determinants and consequences of causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 171–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McArthur, L. A. (1976). The lesser influence of consensus than distinctiveness information on causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 733–742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1951). Personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1953). The achievement motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts (Irvington/Wiley).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1958a). Methods of measuring human motivation. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 7–42). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1958b). Risk taking in children with high and low need for achievement. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 306–321). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1961). The achieving society. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1965). N achievement and entrepreneurship: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 389–392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C. (1970). The two faces of power. Journal of International Affairs, 24, 29–47.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1971). Assessing human motivation. New York: General Learning.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1975). Power: The inner experience. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1976). New introduction. In McClelland, D. C. (Ed.), The achieving society. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1978). Managing motivation to expand human freedom. American Psychologist, 33, 201–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1979). Inhibited power motivation and high blood pressure in men. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 182–190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C. (1980). Motive dispositions: The merits of operant and respondent measures. In Wheeler, L. (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 10–41). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1982). The need for power, sympathetic activation, and illness. Motivation and Emotion, 6, 31–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1984a). Motives as sources of long-term trends in life and health. In McClelland, D. C. (Ed.), Motives, personality, and society. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1984b). The empire-building motivational syndrome. In McClelland, D. C. (Ed.), Motives, personality, and society: Selected papers (pp. 147–174). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1985a). How motives, skills, and values determine what people do. American Psychologist, 41, 812–825.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1985b). Human motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1987a). Human motivation. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1987b). Biological aspects of human motivation. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 11–19). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1989). Motivational factors in health and disease. American Psychologist, 44, 675–683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C. (1995). Achievement motivation in relation to achievement-related recall, performance, and urine flow, a marker associated with release of vasopressin. Motivation and Emotion, 19, 59–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1999). Human motivation (6th ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Alexander, C., & Marks, E. (1982). The need for power, stress, immune function, and illness among male prisoners. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 61–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achievement motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Boyatzis, R. E. (1982). The leadership motive pattern and long term success in management. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 737–743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Clark, R. A., Roby, T. B., & Atkinson, J. W. (1949). The projective expression of need for achievement on thematic apperception. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 242–255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C., Davis, W. N., Kalin, R., & Wanner, E. (1972). The drinking man.New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Davidson, R. J., Floor, E., & Saron, C. (1980). Stressed power motivation, sympathetic activation, immune function, and illness. Journal of Human Stress, 6, 11–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C., Davidson, R. J., Saron, C., & Floor, E. (1980). The need for power, brain norepinephrine turnover, and learning. Biological Psychology, 10, 93–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Franz, C. E. (1992). Motivational and other sources of work accomplishment in mid-life: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality, 60, 680–707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Jemmott, J. B. (1980). Power motivation, stress, and physical illness. Journal of Human Stress, 6, 6–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ?Psychological Review, 96, 690–702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Liberman, A. M. (1949). The effects of need for achievement on recognition of need related words. Journal of Personality, 18, 236–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Locke, S. E., Jemmot, J. B., Kraus, L., Williams, R. M., & Valeri, C. R. (1985). Motivational syndromes associated with natural killer cell activity and illness. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Maddocks, J. A., & McAdams, D. P. (1985). The need for power, brain noradrenaline turnover, and memory. Motivation and Emotion, 9, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Miron, D. (1979). The impact of achievement motivation training in small businesses. California Management Review, 21 (4).Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Patel, V., Stier, D., & Brown, D. (1987). The relationship of affiliative arousal to dopamine release. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 51–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Pilon, D. A. (1983). Sources of adult motives in patterns of parent behavior in early childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 564–574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Ross, G., & Patel, V. (1985). The effect of an academic examination on salivary norepinephrine and immunoglobuline levels. Journal of Human Stress, 11, 52–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Teague, G. (1975). Predicting risk preference among power related tasks. Journal of Personality, 43, 266–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., & Watson, R. I. (1973). Power motivation and risk-taking behavior. Journal of Personality, 41, 121–139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, D. C., & Winter, D. G. (1969). Motivating economic achievement. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1987). Validation of the five factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 81–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997). Conceptions and correlates of openness to experience. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 826–848). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Hrebickova, M., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., Avia, M. D., Sanz, J., Sanchez-Bernardos, M. L., Kusdil, M. E., Woodfield, R., Saunders, P. R., & Smith, P. B. (2000). Nature over nurture: Temperament, personality and life span development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78 (1), 173–186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDougall, W. (1908). An introduction to social psychology. London: Methuen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDougall, W. (1932). The energies of men. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
McGuire, W. J. (1966). The current status of cognitive consistency theories. In Feldman, S. (Ed.), Cognitive consistency (pp. 1–46). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
McKeachie, W. J. (1961). Motivation, teaching methods, and college learning. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1961 (pp. 111–142). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
McReynolds, P. (Ed.). (1971). Advances on psychological assessment (Vol. 2). Palo Alto, CA: Science and Behavior Books.Google Scholar
Meaney, M., Aitken, D., Berkel, C., Bhatnagar, S., & Sapolsky, R. (1988). Effect of neonatal handling on age-related impairments associated with the hippocampus. Science, 239, 766–768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehrabian, A. (1969). Measures of achieving tendency. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 29, 445–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehrabian, A. (1970). The development and validation of measures of affiliative tendency and sensitivity to rejection. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 30, 417–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehrabian, A. (1972). Nonverbal communication. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.Google Scholar
Mehrabian, A., & Ksionzky, S. (1974). A theory of affiliation. Lexington, MA: Heath.Google Scholar
Meins, E. (1999). Sensitivity, security, and internal working models: Bridging the transmission gap. Attachment and Human Development, 1, 325–342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior, 17, 83–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mertin, M. (1994). Aufmerksamkeitszuwendung bei vorgenommenen Gelegenheiten [Focusing attention as intended]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich, Germany.Google Scholar
Metalsky, G. I., Halberstadt, L. J., & Abramson, L. Y. (1987). Vulnerability to depressive mood reactions: Toward a more powerful test of the diatheses – stress and causal mediation components of the reformulated theory of depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 386–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metalsky, G. I., Joiner, T. E., Hardin, T. S., & Abramson, L. Y. (1993). Depressive reactions to failure in a naturalistic setting: A test of the hopelessness and self-esteem theories of depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 101–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metcalfe, J., & Jacobs, W. J. (1998). Emotional memory: The effects of stress on “cool” and “hot” memory systems. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 38, 187– 222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meumann, E. (1908/1913). Intelligenz und Wille [Intelligence and volition]. Leipzig, Germany: Quelle & Meyer.Google Scholar
Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L. D., Kay, G. G., Moreland, K. L., Dies, R. R., Eisman, E. J., Kubiszyn, T. W., & Reed, G. M. (2001). Psychological testing and psychological assessment: A review of evidence and issues. American Psychologist, 56, 128–165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, W.-U. (1972). Überlegungen zur Konstruktion eines Fragebogens zur Erfassung von Selbstkonzepten der Begabung [Thoughts on the construction of a questionnaire to assess self-concepts of ability]. Unpublished manuscript, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1973a). Anstrengungsintention in Abhängigkeit von Begabungseinschätzung und Aufgabenschwierigkeit [Intended effort as a function of evaluations of ability and task difficulty]. Archiv für Psychologie, 125, 245–262.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1973b). Leistungsmotiv und Ursachenerklärung von Erfolg und Mißerfolg [The achievement motive and causal attribution of success and failure]. Stuttgart, Germany: Klett.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1976). Leistungsorientiertes Verhalten als Funktion von wahrgenommener eigener Begabung und wahrgenommener Aufgabenschwierigkeit [Achievement-oriented behavior as a function of self-concept of ability and perceived task difficulty]. In Schmalt, H.-D. & Meyer, W.-U. (Eds.), Leistungsmotivation und Verhalten (pp. 101–135). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1978). Der Einfluss von Sanktionen auf Begabungsperzeptionen [The influence of sanctions on perceptions of own ability] (pp. 71–87). Bielefelder Symposium über Attribution. Bielefeld, Germany.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1984a). Das Konzept von der eigenen Begabung [Self-concept of ability]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1984b). Das Konzept von der eigenen Begabung: Auswirkungen, Stabilität und vorauslaufende Bedingungen [Self-concept of ability: Effects, stability, and antecedent conditions]. Psychologische Rundschau, 35, 136–150.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1987). Perceived ability and achievement-related behavior. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 73–86). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, W.-U. (1988). Die Rolle von Überraschung im Attributionsprozeß [The role of surprise in the attribution process]. Psychologische Rundschau, 39, 136–147.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Heckhausen, H., & Kemmler, L. (1965). Validierungskorrelate der inhaltsanalytisch erfaßten Leistungsmotivation guter und schwacher Schüler des dritten Schuljahres [Validation correlates of the achievement motivation of good and poor third grade students as measured by picture content analysis]. Psychologische Forschung, 28, 301–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Mittag, W., & Engler, U. (1986). Some effects of praise and blame on perceived ability and affect. Social Cognition, 4, 293–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Niepel, M., Rudolph, U., & Schützwohl, A. (1991). An experimental analysis of surprise. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 295–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Niepel, M., & Schützwohl, A. (1994). Überraschung und Attribution [Surprise and attribution]. In Försterling, F. & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (Eds.), Attribution: Grundlagen und Anwendungen (pp. 105–122). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Reisenzein, R., & Dickhäuser, O. (2004). Inferring ability from blame: Effects of effort- versus liking-oriented cognitive schemata. Psychology Science, 46, 281–293.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Schützwohl, A., & Reisenzein, R. (1993). Einführung in die Emotionspsychologie: Bd. 1. Die Emotionstheorien von Watson, James und Schachter [Introduction to the psychology of emotion: Vol. 1. The emotion theories of Watson, James und Schachter]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., Schützwohl, A., & Reisenzein, R. (1999). Einführung in die Emotionspsychologie. Bd. II: Evolutionspsychologische Emotionstheorien [Introduction to the psychology of emotion: Vol. 2. Evolutionary psychology theories of emotion]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Meyer, W.-U., & Starke, E. (1982). Seeking information about one's own ability in relation to self-concept of ability: A field study. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 501–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michalski, N. (2004). Die Wirkung von Vorsätzen in kritischen Situationen im Tenniswettkampf [The effects of implementation intentions in critical situations in tennis competitions]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Konstanz, Germany.Google Scholar
Michotte, A. E. (1912). Note à propos de contributions recentes à la psychologie de la volonté. Études de Psychologie, 1, 193–233.Google Scholar
Michotte, A. E. (1954). Autobiographie. Extrait de “Psychologica Belgia.”Louvain, Belgium: Editions Nauwelaerts.Google Scholar
Michotte, A. E., & Prüm, E. (1910). Étude éxperimentale sur le choix volontaire et ses antecédents immediats. Archives de Psychologie, 10, 119–299.Google Scholar
Midlarsky, E. (1968). Aiding responses: An analysis and review. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 14, 229–260.Google Scholar
Mierke, K. (1955). Wille und Leistung [Volition and achievement]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Milana, S. A. (1981). The effects of naturally occurring depression and induced mood states on social skill. Dissertation Abstracts International, 42, 2541.Google Scholar
Milburn, M. A. (1978). Sources of bias in the prediction of future events. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 21, 17–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. T. (1985). A developmental study of the cognitive basis of performance impairment after failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 529–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. T. (1987). Changes in self academic self concept in early school years: The role of conceptions of ability. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 2, 551–558.Google Scholar
Miller, A. T., & Hom, H. (1990). Influence of extrinsic and ego incentive value on persistence after failure and continuing motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 539–545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. T., & Hom, H. (1997). Conceptions of ability and the interpretation of praise, blame and material rewards. Journal of Experimental Education, 65, 163–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, D. T. (1976). Ego involvement and attribution for success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 901–906.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, D. T., & Ross, M. (1975). Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: Fact or fiction?Psychological Bulletin, 82, 213–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, K. S., & Worchel, P. (1956). The effects of need-achievement and self-ideal discrepancy on performance under stress. Journal of Personality, 25, 176–190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, N. E. (1941). An experimental investigation of acquired drives. Psychological Bulletin, 38, 534–535.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1944). Experimental studies of conflict. In Hunt, J. M. (Ed.), Personality and the behavioral disorders (Vol. I, pp. 431–465). New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1948). Studies of fear as an acquirable drive: I. Fear as motivation and fear-reduction as reinforcement in the learning of new responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 89–101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, N. E. (1951). Learnable drives and rewards. In Stevens, S. S. (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology (pp. 435–472). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1956). Effects of drugs on motivation: The value of using a variety of measures. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 65, 318–333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, N. E. (1959). Liberalization of basic S-R concepts: Extensions to conflict behavior, motivation, and social learning. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. II, pp. 196–292). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1961). Analytical studies of drive and reward. American Psychologist, 16, 739–754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1963). Some reflections on the law of effect produce a new alternative to drive reduction. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1963 (pp. 65–112). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. (1941). Social learning and imitation. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Milne, S., Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2002). Combining motivational and volitional interventions to promote exercise participation: Protection motivation theory and implementation intentions. British Journal of Health Psychology, 7, 163–184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, P. (1970). Physiological psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Mischel, T. (1970). Wundt and the conceptual foundations of psychology. Philosophical and Phenomenological Research, 31, 1–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W., Cantor, N., & Feldman, S. (1996). Principles of self-regulation: The nature of willpower and self-control. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 329–360). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., & Gilligan, C. (1964). Delay of gratification, motivation for the prohibited gratification, and responses to temptation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 411–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246–268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1998). Reconciling processing dynamics and personality dispositions. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 229–258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Smith, R. E. (2004). Introduction to personality (7th ed.). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Mitchell, T. R. (1974). Expectancy models of job satisfaction, occupational preference and effort: A theoretical, methodological, and empirical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 1053–1077.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, T. R. (1982). Expectancy-value models in organizational psychology. In Feather, N. T. (Ed.), Expectations and actions: Expectancy-value models in psychology (pp. 293–312). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mitchell, T. R., & Albright, D. (1972). Expectancy theory predictions of job satisfaction, job effort, job performance, and retention of naval aviation officers. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 8, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, T. R., & Biglan, A. (1971). Instrumentality theories: Current uses in psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 432–454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mittag, H.-D. (1955). Über personale Bedingungen des Gedächtnisses für Handlungen [On the personal conditions of memory for actions]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 158, 40–120.Google Scholar
Modell, J. (1980). Normative aspects of American marriage timing since World War II. Journal of Family History, 5, 210–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Modell, J., Fürstenberg, F. F. J., & Hershberg, T. (1976). Social change and transitions to adulthood in historical perspective. Journal of Family History, 1, 7–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Modell, J., Fürstenberg, F. F. J., & Strong, D. (1978). The timing of marriage in the transition to adulthood: Continuity and change. American Journal of Sociology, 84, 120–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mogenson, G. J., Jones, D. L., & Yim, C. Y. (1980). From motivation to action: Functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. Progress in Neurobiology, 14, 69–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2000). Meaning and motivation. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 131–159). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Moller, A. C., & Elliot, A. J. (2006). The 2 × 2 achievement goal framework: An overview of empirical research. In Mitel, A. V. (Ed.), Focus on educational psychology research (pp. 307–326). New York: Nova Science.Google Scholar
Moneta, G. B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). The effect of perceived challenges and skills on the quality of subjective experience. Journal of Personality, 64, 274–310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monson, T. C., & Snyder, M. (1977). Actors, observers, and the attribution process. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 89–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, C. D., & Murray, H. A. (1935). A method for investigating fantasies: The Thematic Apperceptive Test. Archives of Neurological Psychiatry, 34, 289–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, C. T. (1943). Physiological psychology.New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Morris, J. S., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393(6684), 467–470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moruzzi, G., & Magoun, H. W. (1949). Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. EEG and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1, 455–473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mosher, D. (1968). Measurement of guilt in females by self-report inventories. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 32, 690–695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moulton, R. W. (1958). Notes for a projective measure for fear of failure. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 563–571). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Moulton, R. W. (1965). Effects of success and failure on level of aspiration as related to achievement motives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 399–406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mowrer, H. O. (1939). A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent. Psychological Review, 46, 553–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mowrer, H. O. (1947). On the dual nature of learning: A reinterpretation of “conditioning” and “problem-solving.”Harvard Educational Review, 17, 102–148.Google Scholar
Mowrer, H. O. (1960). Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, G. E., & Pilzecker, A. (1900). Experimentelle Beiträge zur Lehre vom Gedächtnis [Experimental contributions to memory research]. Leipzig, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
Münsterberg, H. (1888). Die Willenshand lung: Ein Beitrag der physiologischen Psychologie [Volitional acts: A contribution to physiological psychology]. Freiburg, Germany: Moler.Google Scholar
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle?Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774–789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, E. J., & Berkun, M. M. (1955). Displacement as a function of conflict. Social Psychology, 51, 47–56.Google ScholarPubMed
Murray, H. A. (1933). The effect of fear upon estimates of the maliciousness of other personalities. Journal of Social Psychology, 4, 310–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality.New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic Apperceptive Test Manual.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Murstein, B. I., & Pryer, R. S. (1959). The concept of projection: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 353–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, J. (1991). Optimales Erleben und die Nutzung der Begabung [Optimal experience and making use of talent]. In Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.), Die außergewöhnliche Erfahrung im Alltag: Die Psychologie des Flow-Erlebens (pp. 326–334). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Neiss, R. (1988). Reconceptualizing arousal: Psychobiological states in motor performance. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 345–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, E. E., & Panksepp, J. (1998). Brain substrates of infant-mother attachment: Contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 22(3), 437–452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, R. J. (2005). An introduction to behavioral endocrinology (3rd ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Nesse, R. M. (2000). Is depression an adaptation?Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 14–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesse, R. M. (2001). Evolution and the capacity for commitment. Volume III in the Russell Sage Foundation Series on Trust. New York: Sage.Google Scholar
Neugarten, B. L. (1979). Time, age, and the life cycle. American Journal of Psychiatry, 136, 887–894.Google ScholarPubMed
Neugarten, B. L., Moore, J. W., & Lowe, J. C. (1965). Age norms, age constraints and adult socialization. American Journal of Sociology, 70, 710–717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Newell, A., & Rosenbloom, P. S. (1981). Mechanisms of skill acquisition and the law of practice. In Anderson, J. R. (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 1–55). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Newtson, D. (1974). Dispositional inference from effects of actions: Effects chosen and effects foregone. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 489–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1975). Causal attributions and other achievement-related cognitions: Effects of task outcome, attainment value, and sex. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 379–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1976). Effort is virtuous, but it's better to have ability: Evaluative responses to perceptions of effort and ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 306–315.Google Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1978). The development of the concepts of effort and ability, perception of own attainment, and the understanding that difficult tasks require more than ability. Child Development, 49, 800–814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1984a). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1984b). Conceptions of ability and achievement motivation. In Ames, R. & Ames, C. (Eds.), Student motivation (pp. 39–73). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1985). Development and its discontents: The differentiation of the concept of ability. In Nicholls, J. G. (Ed.), The development of achievement motivation (pp. 185–218). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nicholls, J. G., & Miller, A. T. (1983). The differentiation of the concepts of difficulty and ability. Child Development, 54, 951–959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niederberger, U., Engemann, A., & Radtke, M. (1987). Umfang der Informationsverarbeitung bei Entscheidungen: Der Einfluss von Gedächtnisbelastung und Handlungsorientierung [Scope of information processing in decision making: The impact of load and action orientation]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 34, 80–100.Google Scholar
Nietzsche, F. (1878). Menschliches, allzumenschliches [Human, all-too-human] (H. Zimmern, Trans.). Dover Publication.Google Scholar
Niitamo, P. (1999). “Surface” and “depth” in human personality: Relations between explicit and implicit motives. Helsinki, Finland: Finish Institute of Occupational Health.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Borgida, E. (1975). Attribution and the psychology of prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 932–943.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Borgida, E., Crandall, R., & Reed, H. (1976). Popular induction: Information is not necessarily informative. In Carroll, J. & Payne, J. (Eds.), Cognitive and social behavior (pp. 113–133). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolen-Hoecksema, S., Girgus, J. S., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1992). Predictors and consequences of childhood symptoms: A 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 405–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 92–104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, D. A. (1980). Twelve issues for cognitive science. Cognitive Science, 4, 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In Davidson, R. J., Schwartz, G. E., & Shapiro, D. (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research (Vol. 4, pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novak, T. P., & Hoffman, D. L. (1997, July). Measuring the flow experience among web users. Paper presented at the Interval Research Corporation, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee.Google Scholar
Nurmi, J. E. (1992). Age differences in adult life goals, concerns and their temporal extension: A life course approach to future-oriented motivation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 487–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nurmi, J. E., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2002). Goal construction, reconstruction and depressive symptoms in a life-span context: The transition from school to work. Journal of Personality, 70, 385–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nurmi, J. E., Salmela-Aro, K., & Koivisto, P. (2002). Goal importance and related achievement beliefs and emotions during the transition from vocational school to work: Antecedents and consequences. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60, 241–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nygard, R. (1975). A reconsideration of the achievement motivation theory. European Journal of Social Psychology, 5, 61–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nygard, R. (1977). Personality, situation, and persistence.Oslo, Norway: Universitetsforlaget.Google Scholar
Nygard, R. (1982). Achievement motives and individual differences in situational specificity of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 319–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1215–1229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, P., Atkinson, J. W., & Horner, M. S. (1966). Motivational implications of ability grouping in schools. In Atkinson, J. W. & Feather, N. T. (Eds.), A theory of achievement motivation (pp. 231–248). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
O'Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., & Andrews, C. (2001). Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 4(1), 95–102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oettingen, G. (1996). Positive fantasy and motivation. In Gollwitzer, P. M. & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), Psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 236–259). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Oettingen, G. (1997). Psychologie des Zukunftsdenkens [The psychology of thinking about the future]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Oettingen, G. (2000). Expectancy effects on behavior depend on self-regulatory thought. Social Cognition, 18, 101–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2000). Das Setzen und Verwirklichen von Zielen [Goal setting and goal striving]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 208(3–4), 406–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Goal setting and goal striving. In Tesser, A. & Schwarz, N. (Eds.), The Blackwell handbook of social psychology (pp. 329–347). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Oettingen, G., Pak, H. J., & Schnetter, K. (2001). Self-regulation of goal-setting: Turning free fantasies about the future into binding goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 736–753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Öhman, A., Fredrikson, M., Hughdal, K., & Rimmö, P. A. (1976). The premise of equipotentiality in human classical conditioning: Conditioned electrodermal responses to potentially phobic stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105, 313–337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olds, J. (1955). Physiological mechanisms of reward. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1955 (pp. 73–139). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Olds, J. (1958). Satiation effects in self-stimulation of the brain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 51, 675–679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olds, J. (1969). The central nervous system and the reinforcement of behavior. American Psychologist, 24, 114–132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olds, J., & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 47, 419–427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olds, J., & Olds, M. (1965). Drives, rewards, and the brain. In Newcomb, T. N. (Ed.), New directions in psychology (Vol. II, pp. 327–404). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Olweus, D. (1976). Der “moderne” Interaktionismus von Person und Situation und seine varianzanalytische Sackgasse [The “modern” interactionism of person and situation and the impasse of variance analysis]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 8, 171–185.Google Scholar
Orbell, S. (2003). Personality systems interaction theory and the theory of planned behavior: Evidence that self-regulatory volitional components enhance enactment of studying behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 95–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orbell, S., Hodgkins, S., & Sheeran, P. (1997). Implementation intentions and the theory of planned behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 945–954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2000). Motivational and volitional processes in action initiation: A field study of the role of implementation intentions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 780–797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orpen, C. (1983). Risk-taking attitudes among Indian, United States, and Japanese managers. Journal of Social Psychology, 120, 283–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orvis, B. R., Cunningham, J. D., & Kelley, H. H. (1975). A closer examination of causal inference: The role of consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 605–616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oster, G., & Alberch, P. (1982). Evolution and bifurcation of developmental programs. Evolution, 36, 444–459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ovsiankina, M. (1928). Die Wiederaufnahme unterbrochener Handlungen [The resumption of interrupted activities]. Psychologische Forschung, 11, 302–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyegbile, T. O. and Marler, C. A. (2005). Winning fights elevates testosterone levels in California mice and enhances future ability to win fights. Hormones and Behavior 48, 259–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Packard, M. G., Cornell, A. H., & Alexander, G. M. (1997). Rewarding affective properties of intra-nucleus accumbens injections of testosterone. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 219–224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational Research, 66, 543–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palfai, T. P. (2002). Action-state orientation and the self-regulation of eating behavior. Eating Behaviors, 3, 249–259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palfai, T. P., McNally, A. M., & Roy, M. (2002). Volition and alcohol-risk reduction: The role of action orientation in the reduction of alcohol-related harm among college student drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 27, 309–317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pang, J. S., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2005). Assessing implicit motives in U.S. college students: Effects of picture type and position, gender and ethnicity, and cross-cultural comparisons. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85(3), 280–294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2003). Feeling the pain of social rejection. Science, 302, 237–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papoušek, H. (1967). Experimental studies of appetitional behavior in human newborns and infants. In Stevenson, H. W., Hess, E. H., & Rheingold, H. L. (Eds.), Early behavior: Comparative developmental approaches (pp. 249–277). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Papoušek, H., & Papoušek, M. (1975). Cognitive aspects of preverbal social interaction between human infants and adults. In Hofer, M. (Ed.), Parent-infant interaction (pp. 241–269). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google ScholarPubMed
Papoušek, H., & Papoušek, M. (1987). Intuitive parenting: A dialectic counterpart to the infant's integrative competence. In Osofsky, J. D. (Eds.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 669–720). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Parsons, J. E., & Ruble, D. (1977). The development of achievement-related expectancies. Child Development, 48, 1075–1079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Passman, R. H., & Erck, T. (1978). Permitting maternal contact through vision alone: Films of mothers for promoting play and locomotion. Developmental Psychology, 14(5), 512–516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patten, R. L., & White, L. A. (1977). Independent effects of achievement motivation and overt attribution on achievement behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 39–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, R. (2001). The school-to-work transition: A cross-national perspective. Journal of Economic Literature, 39, 34–92.Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes.London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Peak, H. (1955). Attitude and motivation. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1955 (pp. 149–189). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Pecina, S., Cagniard, B., Berridge, K. C., Aldridge, J. W., & Zhuang, X. (2003). Hyperdopaminergic mutant mice have higher “wanting” but not “liking” for sweet rewards. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(28), 9395–9402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peck, J. W., & Blass, E. M. (1975). Localization of thirst and antidiuretic osmoreceptors by intracranial injections in rats. American Journal of Physiology, 5, 1501–1509.Google Scholar
Pekrun, R. (1993). Entwicklung von schulischer Aufgabenmotivation in der Sekundarstufe: Ein erwartungswert-theoretischer Ansatz [Development of task-related motivation in secondary school: An expectancy-value approach]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 7, 87–98.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. (1993). Putting stress into words: Health, linguistic, and therapeutic implications. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 539–548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penner, D. D., Fitch, G., & Weick, K. E. (1966). Dissonance and the revision of choice criteria. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 701–705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perin, C. I. (1942). Behavioral potentiality as a joint function of the amount of training and the degree of hunger at the time of extinction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30, 93–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perloff, L. S., & Fetzer, B. K. (1986). Self-other judgements and perceived vulnerability of victimization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 502–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérusse, D. (1993). Cultural and reproductive success in industrial societies: Testing the relationship at the proximate and ultimate levels. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 267–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérusse, D. (1994). Mate choice in modern societies: Testing evolutionary hypotheses with behavioural data. Human Nature, 5, 255–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pervin, L. A. (Ed.). (1989). Goal concepts in personality and social psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Peterson, B. E., & Stewart, A. J. (1993). Generativity and social motives in young adults. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 186–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psychological Review, 91, 347–374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, C., Semmel, A., Baeyer, C., Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1982). The attributional style questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6, 287–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfaffmann, C. (1982). Taste: A model of incentive motivation. In Pfaff, D. W. (Ed.), The physiological mechanisms of motivation (pp. 61–97). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfänder, A. (1911). Motive und Motivation [Motives and motivation]. In Pfänder, A. (Ed.), Münchener Philosophische Abhandlungen (Festschrift für Theodor Lipps) (pp. 163–195). Leipzig, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
Pfaus, J. G., Damsma, G., Wenkstern, D., & Fibiger, H. C. (1995). Sexual activity increases dopamine transmission in the nucleus accumbens and striatum of female rats. Brain Research, 693(1–2), 21–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfister, R. (2002). Flow im Alltag [Flow in everyday life]. Berne, Switzerland: Lang.Google Scholar
Phares, E. J. (1976). Locus of control in personality. Morristown, NJ: General Learning.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1936). Le naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant. Neuchâtel, Switzerland: Delachaux et Nestlé.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pikowsky, B. (1988). Lob im Unterricht: Lehrer-und Schülerkognitionen im Vergleich [Praise in school classes: Comparison of teacher and student cognitions]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 2, 251–257.Google Scholar
Pintrich, P. R., & Groot, E. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pintrich, P. R., & Garcia, T. (1991). Student goal orientation and self-regulation in the college classroom. In Maehr, M. & Pintrich, P. R. (Eds.), Advance in motivation and achievement: Goals and self-regulatory processes (Vol. 7, pp. 371–402). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Pizzagalli, D. A., Regard, M., & Lehmann, D. (1999). Rapid emotional face processing in the human right and left brain hemispheres: An ERP study. NeuroReport, 10, 2691–2698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plath, D. W., & Ikeda, K. (1975). After coming of age: Adult awareness of age norms. In Williams, T. R. (Ed.), Socialization and communication in primacy groups (pp. 107–123). The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R. (2004). Genetics and developmental psychology. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 341–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Craig, I. W., & McGuffin, P. (Eds.). (2003). Behavioral genetics in the postgenomic era. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., & Roew, D. C. (1979). Genetic and environmental psychology. Developmental Psychology, 15, 62–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plutchi, R. (1980). Emotion: A psychoevolutionary synthesis.New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Pomerantz, E. M., & Ruble, D. (1997). Distinguishing multiple dimensions of conceptions of ability: Implications for self-evaluations. Child Development, 68, 1165–1180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poppe, P. (2002). Nutzung von Kovariationsinformationen im Attributionsprozess: Ein Vergleich depressiver und nichtdepressiver Erwachsener [Use of covariation information in the attribution process: A comparison of depressive and nondepressive adults]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Gießen, Germany.Google Scholar
Poppe, P., Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Pelster, A. (2005). Attributionsstilfragebogen für Erwachsene (ASF-E) [Attribution style questionnaire for adults (ASF-E)]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Porter, L. W., & Lawler, E. E. (1968). Managerial attitudes and performance. Homewood, IL: Irwin-Dorsey.Google Scholar
Porter, R. H. (1998). Olfaction and human kin recognition. Genetica, 104(3), 259–263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pösl, I. (1994). Wiederaufnahme unterbrochener Handlungen: Effekte der Bewusstseinslagen des Abwägens und Planens [Mindset effects on the resumption of disrupted activities]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Munich, Germany.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1992). Attentional mechanisms and conscious experience. In Milner, A. D. & Rugg, M. D. (Eds.), The neuropsychology of consciousness (pp. 91–111). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Postman, L., & Solomon, R. L. (1949). Perceptual sensitivity to completed and incompleted tasks. Journal of Personality, 18, 347–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulin, M., Haase, C. M., & Heckhausen, J. (2005). Engagement and disengagement across the life-span: An analysis of two process models of development regulation. In Greve, W., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (Eds.), The adaptive self: Personal continuity and intentional self-development (pp. 117–135). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber.Google Scholar
Pribram, K. H. (1976). Self-consciousness and intentionality. In Schwartz, G. E. & Shapiro, D. (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research (Vol. 1, pp. 51–100). New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pritchard, R. D., & Sanders, M. S. (1973). The influence of valence, instrumentality, and expectancy of effort and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 57, 55–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruitt, D. J., & Insko, C. A. (1980). Extension of the Kelley attribution model: The role of comparison-object consensus, target-object consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 39–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puca, R. M. (2001). Preferred difficulty and subjective probability in different action phases. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 307–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puca, R. M., & Schmalt, H. (2001). The influence of the achievement motive on spontaneous thoughts in pre- and postdecisional action phases. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 302–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pusey, A., Williams, J., & Goodall, J. (1997). The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science, 277, 828–831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pyszczynski, T. A., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Toward an integration of cognitive and motivational perspectives on social inference: A biased hypotheses-testing model. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 20). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Radloff, L. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramey, C., & Finkelstein, N. (1978). Contingent stimulation and infant competence. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 3, 89–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rank, O. (1945). Will therapy and truth and reality. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Rapaport, D. (1959). The structure of psychoanalytic theory: A systematizing attempt. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. III, pp. 55–183). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Rapaport, D. (1960). On the psychoanalytic theory of motivation. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1960 (pp. 173–247). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Raps, C. S., Peterson, C., Reinhard, K. E., Abramson, L. Y., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1982). Attributional style among depressed patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 102–108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasch, G. (1960). Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. Copenhagen, Denmark: Nielson & Lydicke.Google Scholar
Raup, R. B. (1925). Complacency, the foundation of human behavior.New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Raven, B. H. (1974). The comparative analysis of power and power preference. In Tedeschi, J. T. (Ed.), Perspectives on social power. Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
Raynor, J. O. (1969). Future orientation and motivation of immediate activity: An elaboration of the theory of achievement motivation. Psychological Review, 76, 606–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raynor, J. O. (1974). Future orientation in the study of achievement motivation. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 121–154). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Raynor, J. O., & Entin, E. E. (1982). Motivation, career striving and aging. Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Raynor, J. O., & Roeder, G. P. (1987). Motivation and future orientation: Task and time effects for achievement motivation. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 61–71). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reif, M. (1970). Leistungsmotivation in Abhängigkeit vom Erziehungsverhalten der Mutter [Achievement motivation as a function of the mother's parenting]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Reitman, W. R. (1960). Motivational induction and the behavioral correlates of the achievement and affiliation motives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 8–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Remy, K. (2000). Entwicklung eines Fragebogens zum Flow-Erleben [Development of a questionnaire on flow experience]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bielefeld, Germany.Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1968). Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 1–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, R. A. (1972). Informational variables in Pavlovian conditioning. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 1–46). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current theory and research (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
Resnik, S. M.Gottesman, I. I., & McGue, M. (1993). Sensation seeking in opposite-sex twins: An effect of prenatal hormones?Behavior Genetics, 23, 323–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rethorts, S. (1994). Attribution und Emotion in der sportpsychologischen Forschung [Attribution and emotion in sports psychology research]. In Försterling, F. & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (Eds.), Attributionstheorie (pp. 163–183). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Reuman, D. A. (1982). Ipsative behavioral variability and the quality of thematic apperceptive measurement of the achievement motive. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1098–1110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reuman, D. A., Alwin, D. F., & Veroff, J. (1984). Assessing the validity of the achievement motive in the presence of random measurement error. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1347–1386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Revelle, W. (1986). Motivation and efficiency of cognitive performance. In Brown, D. R. & Veroff, J. (Eds.), Frontiers of motivational psychology (pp. 107–127). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revelle, W., & Michaels, E. J. (1976). The theory of achievement motivation revisited: The implications of inertial tendencies. Psychological Review, 83, 394–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, W. F., & Anderson, J. E. (1961). Choice behavior in a T-maze as a function of deprivation period and magnitude or reward. Psychological Reports, 8, 131–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1975). Zeitstabilität und Steuerbarkeit von Ursachen schulischer Leistung in der Sicht des Lehrers [Temporal stability and controllability of the causes of school outcomes from the teacher's perspective]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 7, 180–194.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1980). Leistungsbewertung und Lernmotivation [Achievement evaluation and motivation to learn]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1989). Zweck und Tätigkeit [Purpose and activity]. Göttingen, Ger many: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1991). Flow-experience when motorcycling: A study of a special human condition. In Brendicke, R. (Ed.), Safety, environment, future: Proceedings of the 1991 International Motorcycle Conference (pp. 349–362). Bochum, Germany: IfZ.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1993). Anreize engagiert betriebener Freizeitaktivitäten. Ein Systematisierungsversuch [An attempt to systematize the incentives of leisure time activities]. Unpublished manuscript, University of Potsdam, Germany.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1995). Motivation. Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (1996). Flow-Erleben, Freude an riskantem Sport und andere “unvernünftige” Motivationen [Flow experience, enjoyment of dangerous sports and other “unwise” motivations]. In Kuhl, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Motivation, Volition und Handlung. Enzyklopädie der Psychologie C(IV(4 (pp. 101–118). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (2001). Bezugsnormorientierung [Reference norm orientations]. In Rost, D. (Ed.), Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie (pp. 55–62). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (2002a). Freude am Kompetenzerwerb, Flow-Erleben und motivpassende Ziele [Enjoyment of learning, flow experience and motive-congruent goals]. In Salisch, M. V. (Ed.), Emotionale Kompetenz entwickeln (pp. 179–206). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (2002b). Motivationale Kompetenz [Motivational competence]. Paper presented at the 22nd Colloquium on Motivational Psychology (MPK), Siegen, Germany. Retrieved July 12, 2007, from www.psych.uni-potsdam.de/people/rheinberg/personal/lectures-d.html.
Rheinberg, F. (2004a). Motivationsdiagnostik [Measurement of motivation]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. (2004b, July). Motivational competence and flow-experience. Paper presented at the 2nd European Conference of Positive Psychology, Verbania, Italy. Retrieved July 12, 2007, from www.psych.uni-potsdam.de/people/rheinberg/personal/lectures-d.html.
Rheinberg, F. (2006). Motivation (6th ed.). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Duscha, R., & Michels, U. (1980). Zielsetzung und Kausalattribution in Abhängigkeit vom Leistungsvergleich [Goal setting and causal attribution as a function of performance comparisons]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 12, 177– 189.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Iser, I., & Pfauser, S. (1997). Freude am Tun und(oder zweckorientiertes Schaffen? Zur transsituativen Konsistenz und konvergenten Validität der Anreiz-Fokus-Skala [Doing something for fun and/or for gain? Transsituational consistency and convergent validity of the Incentive Focus Scale]. Diagnostica, 42, 174–191.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., & Krug, S. (2005). Motivationsförderung im Schulalltag [Fostering motivation in school] (3rd ed.). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Lührmann, J.-V., & Wagner, H. (1977). Bezugsnormorientierung von Schülern der 5. bis 13. Klassenstufe bei der Leistungsbeurteilung [Reference norm orientations in 5th to 13th graders]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogischer Psychologie, 9, 90–93.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., & Manig, Y. (2003). Was macht Spaß am Graffiti-Sprayen? Eine induktive Anreizanalyse [What is enjoyable about graffiti spraying? An inductive incentive analysis]. Report Psychologie, 4, 222–234.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Manig, Y., Kliegl, R., Engeser, S., & Vollmeyer, R. (2007). Flow bei der Arbeit, doch Glück in der Freizeit: Zielausrichtung, Flow und Glücksgefühle [Flow at work, but happiness at leisure: Goal directedness, flow and feelings of happiness]. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 51, 105–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Schmalt, H., & Wasser, I. (1978). Ein Lehrerunterschied, der etwas ausmacht [A difference between teachers that matters]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 10(1), 3–7.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F. & Tramp, N. (2006) Anreizanalyse intensiver Nutzung von Computen in der Freizeit [Analysis of the incentive for intensive leisure-time use of computers]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 214, 97–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rheinberg, F., & Vollmeyer, R. (2003). Flow-Erleben in einem Computerspiel unter experimentell variierten Bedingungen [Flow experience in a computer game under experimentally controlled conditions]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 114, 161–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rheinberg, F., & Vollmeyer, R. (2004). Flow-Erleben bei der Arbeit und in der Freizeit [Flow experience at work and in leisure time]. In Wegge, J. & Schmidt, K.-H. (Eds.), Förderung von Arbeitsmotivation und Gesundheit in Organisationen (pp. 163–180). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Burns, B. D. (2000). Motivation and self-regulated learning. In Heckhausen, J. (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 81–108). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Engeser, S. (2003). Die Erfassung des Flow-Erlebens [Measuring flow experience]. In Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. & Rheinberg, F. (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept(Tests and Trends Vol. 2, pp. 261–279). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Rheinberg, F., & Weich, K. (1988). Wie gefährlich ist Lob? Eine Untersuchung zum “paradoxen Effekt” von Lehrersanktionen [How risky is it to praise students? A study concerning “paradox effects” of teachers' sanctions]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 2, 227–233.Google Scholar
Rheingold, H., & Eckerman, C. (1969). The infant's free entry into a new environment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8(2), 271–283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rholes, W. S., Blackwell, J., Jordan, C., & Walters, C. (1980). A developmental study of learned helplessness. Developmental Psychology, 16, 616–624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rholes, W. S., &. Ruble, D. (1984). Children's understanding of dispositional characteristics of others. Child Development, 55, 550–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richter, C. P. (1927). Animal behavior and internal drives. Quarterly Review of Biology, 2, 307–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1997). Genetic and environmental influences on personality: A study of twins reared together using the self- and peer-report NEO-FFI scales. Journal of Personality, 65, 449–475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (1978). Effects of caregiver behavior on habituation rate and self-efficacy in infants. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1, 105–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rind, B., & Kipnis, D. (1999). Changes in self-perception as a result of successfully persuading others. Journal of Social Issues, 55, 141–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rindfuss, R. R., Swicegood, C. G., & Rosenfeld, R. A. (1987). Disorder in the life course: How common and does it matter?American Sociological Review, 52, 785–801.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rinn, W. E. (1984). The neuropsychology of facial emotions: A review of the neurological and psychological mechanism for producing facial expressions. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 52–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riskind, J. H. (1984). They stoop to conquer: Guiding and self-regulatory functions of physical posture after success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 479–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, B. W., & Caspi, A. (2003). The cumulative continuity model of personality development: Striking a balance between continuity and change in personality traits across the life course. Understanding human development: Dialogues with lifespan psychology (pp. 183–214). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2000). The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: An incentive-sensitization view. Addiction, 95(Suppl 2), S91–S117.Google Scholar
Roe, A., & Siegelman, M. (1964). The origin of interests. APGA Inquiry Studies, 1, 98.Google Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person: A therapist's view of psychotherapy.Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Rogers, E. M., & Svenning, L. (1969). Modernization among peasants: The impact of communication.New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 124, 207–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B., & Wertsch, J.(1984). Children's learning in the “zone of proximal development.” New Directions for Child Development, 23.Google Scholar
Röhrle, B., Hedke, J., & Leibold, S. (1994). Persönliche Projekte zur Herstellung und Pflege sozialer Beziehungen bei depressiven und nicht-depressiven Personen [Personal projects serving the establishment and maintenance of social relations in depressive and nondepressive adults]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, 23, 43–51.Google Scholar
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values.New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and emotion. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 284–294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 11–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, E. T. (2005). Taste, olfactory, and food texture processing in the brain, and the control of food intake. Physiology & Behavior, 85(1), 45–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roney, J. R., Mahler, S. V., & Maestripieri, D. (2003). Behavioral and hormonal responses of men to brief interactions with women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 365–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rook, K., Sorkin, D., & Zettel, A. (2004). Stress in social relationships: Coping and adaptation across the lifespan. In Lang, F. R. & Fingerman, K. L. (Eds.), Growing together: Personal relationships across the lifespan (pp. 210–239). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rosahl, S. K., Tennigkeit, M., Kuhl, J., & Haschke, R. (1993). Handlungskontrolle und langsame Hirnpotentiale: Untersuchungen zum Einfluss subjektiv kritischer Wörter (Erste Ergebnisse) [Action control and slow brain potentials: Studies on the impact of subjectively emotional words (first findings)]. Zeitschrift für Medizinische Psychologie, 2, 1–8.Google Scholar
Rose, M. (1991). Evolutionary biology of aging. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rose, R. M., Holaday, J. W., & Bernstein, I. S. (1971). Plasma testosterone, dominance rank, and aggressive behavior in male rhesus monkeys. Nature, 231, 898–904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, B. C. (1959). Race, ethnicity, and the achievement syndrome. American Sociological Review, 24, 47–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosen, B. C., & Andrade, D' R. (1959). The psychosocial origins of achievement motivation. Sociometry, 22, 185–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenbaum, R. M. (1972). A dimensional analysis of the perceived causes of success and failure. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, G. J. (1956). Cognitive structure and attitudinal affect. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53, 367–372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenzweig, S. (1933). Preferences in the repetition of successful and unsuccessful activities as a function of age and personality. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 42, 423–441.Google Scholar
Rosenzweig, S. (1941). Need-persistive and ego-defensive reactions to frustration as demonstrated by an experiment on repression. Psychological Review, 48, 347–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenzweig, S. (1943). Experimental study of “repression” with special reference to need-persistive and ego-defensive reactions to frustration. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 64–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenzweig, S. (1945). Further comparative data on repetition choice after success and failure as related to frustration tolerance. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 66, 75–81.Google Scholar
Rösler, H.-D. (1955). Über das Behalten von Handlungen schwachsinniger und normaler Kinder [Retention of actions by feeble-minded and normal children]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 158, 161–231.Google Scholar
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his short-comings: Distortions in the attribution process. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173–220). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rotenberg, V. S. (1993). Richness against freedom: Two hemisphere functions and the problem of creativity. European Journal for High Ability, 4, 11–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotenberg, V. S. (2004). The peculiarity of the right hemisphere function in depression: Solving the paradoxes. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 28, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothermund, K., & Brandstädter, J. (2003a). Coping with deficits and losses in later life: From compensatory action to accommodation. Psychology and Aging, 18, 896–905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothermund, K., & Brandstädter, J. (2003b). Depression in later life: Cross sequential patterns and possible determinants. Psychology and Aging, 18, 80–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J. B. (1954). Social learning and clinical psychology.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J. B. (1955). The role of the psychological situation in determining the direction of human behavior. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1955 (pp. 245–269). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Rotter, J. B. (1960). Some implications of a social learning theory for the prediction of goal directed behavior from testing procedures. Psychological Review, 67, 301–316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80(1, Whole no. 609), 1–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J. B. (1982). Social learning theory. In Feather, N. T. (Ed.), Expectations and actions (pp. 241–260). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rotter, J. B., Chance, J. E., & Phares, E. J. (1972). Applications of a social learning theory of personality.New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Rotter, J. B., Liverant, S., & Crowne, D. P. (1961). The growth and extinction of expectancies in chance controlled and skilled tasks. Journal of Psychology, 52, 161–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J. B., Seeman, M. R., & Liverant, S. (1962). Internal versus external control of reinforcements: A major variable in behavior theory. In Washburn, W. F. (Ed.), Decisions, values, and groups (Vol. 2, pp. 473–516). New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Rovee, C. K., & Fagan, J. (1976). Extended conditioning and 24 hour retention in infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P. (1976). The evolution of intelligence and access to the cognitive unconscious. In Sprague, J. M. & Epstein, A. N. (Eds.), Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology (pp. 245–277). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ruble, D. N., Boggiano, A., Feldman, N. S., & Loebl, J. H. (1980). A developmental analysis of the role of social comparison in self-evaluation. Developmental Psychology, 16, 105–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruble, D. N., & Feldman, N. S. (1976). Order of consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency information and causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 930–937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (2002). Conceptualization, measuring and evaluation the developmental course of gender differentiation: Compliments, queries and quandaries. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(269), 148–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruble, D. N., Parsons, J. E., & Ross, M. (1976). Self-evaluative responses of children in an achievement setting. Child Development, 48, 1362–1368.Google Scholar
Rudolph, U., Roesch, S. C., Weiner, B., & Greitmeyer, T. (2004). Responsibility and help-giving: A meta-analytic review. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 815–848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruhland, D., & Feld, S. (1977). The development of achievement motivation in black and white children. Child Development, 48, 1362–1368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, B. (1938). Power.London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 3–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). When rewards compete with nature: The undermining of intrinsic motivation and self-regulation. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 14–54). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M., Kuhl, J., & Deci, E. (1997). Nature and autonomy: An organizational view of social and neurobiological aspects of self-regulation in behavior and development. Development and Psychopathology, 9(4), 701–728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadalla, E. K., Kenrick, D. T., & Vershure, B. (1987). Dominance and heterosexual attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 730–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sader, M., & Keil, W. (1968). Faktorenanalytische Untersuchungen zur Projektion der Leistungsmotivation [Factor analytic studies on the projection of achievement motivation]. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 120, 25–53.Google Scholar
Sader, M., & Specht, H. (1967). Leistung, Motivation und Leistungsmotivation: Korrelationsstatistische Untersuchungen zur Leistungsmotivmessung nach Heckhausen [Achievement, motivation and achievement motivation: Correlational studies on achievement motivation after Heckhausen]. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 119, 90–130.Google Scholar
Salamone, J. D. (1994). The involvement of nucleus accumbens dopamine in appetitive and aversive motivation. Behavioural Brain Research, 61, 117–133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sameroff, A. J. (1983). Developmental systems: Contexts and evolution. Handbook of Child Psychology, 1(4), 237–294.Google Scholar
Sanford, R. N. (1937). The effects of abstinence from food upon imaginal processes: A preliminary experiment. Journal of Psychology, 3, 145–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanford, R. N., & Risser, J. (1948). What are the conditions of self-defensive forgetting?Journal of Personality, 17, 244–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sansone, C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sansone, C., & Smith, J. L. (2000). Interest and self-regulation: The relation between having to and wanting to. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 343–372). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sansone, C. D., Sachau, A., & Weir, C. (1989). Effects of instruction on intrinsic interest: The importance of context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 819–829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapolsky, R. M. (1987). Stress, social status, and reproductive physiology in free-living baboons. In Crews, D. (Ed.), Psychobiology and reproductive behavior: An evolutionary perspective (pp. 291–322). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Sapolsky, R. M. (1992). Stress, the aging brain, and the mechanism of neuron death. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. (1996). The language of personality: Lexical reflections on the five-factor model. In Wiggins, J. S. (Ed.), The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives(pp. 21–50). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sawusch, J. R. (1974). Computer simulation of the influence of ability and motivation on test performance and cumulative achievement and the relation between them. In Atkinson, J. W. & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 425–438). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424–435.Google ScholarPubMed
Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501–518.Google Scholar
Schallberger, U. (2000). Qualität des Erlebens in Arbeit und Freizeit: Eine Zwischenbilanz [Quality of experience in work and leisure: An interim assessment]. Berichte aus der Abteilung Angewandte Psychologie, No. 31. Zürich, Switzerland: University of Zürich.Google Scholar
Schallberger, U., & Pfister, R. (2001). Flow-Erleben in Arbeit und Freizeit: Eine Untersuchung zum Paradox der Arbeit mit der Experience Sampling Method [Flow experiences in work and leisure: An experience sampling study about the paradox of work]. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 45, 176–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheffer, D. (2000). Entwicklungsbedingungen Impliziter Motive: Bindung, Leistung und Macht [Developmental conditions of implicit motives: Affiliation, achievement, and power]. Doctoral dissertation, University of Osnabrück, Germany. Retrieved July 12, 2007, from http://elib.ub.uni-osnabrueck.de/publications/diss/E-Diss150_thesis.pdf.Google Scholar
Scheffer, D. (2003). Die Messung impliziter Motive [The measurement of implicit motives]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Scheffer, D. (2005). Implizite Motive [Implicit motives]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Scheffer, D., & Kuhl, J. (2006). Erfolgreich motivieren [Successful motivation]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Scheffer, D., Kuhl, J., & Eichstaedt, J. (2003). Der Operante Motiv-Test (OMT): Inhaltsklassen, Auswertung, psychometrische Kennwerte und Validierung [The Operant Motive Test (OMT): Categories, interpretation, psychometric properties, and validation]. In Rheinberg, F. & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept (pp. 151–168). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4, 219–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R. (1981). Über die Vernachlässigung der Emotion in der Psychologie [On the neglect of emotion in psychology]. In Michaelis, M. (Ed.), Bericht über den 32. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie, Zürich 1980 (pp. 304–317). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In Scherer, K. R. & Ekman, P. (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293–317). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schiefele, U. (1996). Motivation und Lernen mit Texten [Motivation and learning from texts]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schiefele, U., & Köller, O. (2001). Intrinsische und extrinsische Motivation [Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation]. In Rost, D. H. (Ed.), Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie (pp. 304–310). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Schiefele, U., & Rheinberg, F. (1997). Motivation and knowledge acquisition: Searching for mediating processes. In Maehr, M. L. & Pintrich, P. R. (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10, pp. 251–301). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Schiefele, U., & Urhahne, D. (2000). Motivationale und volitionale Bedingungen der Studienleistung [Motivational and volitional conditions of college outcomes]. In Schiefele, U. & Wild, K.-P. (Eds.), Interesse und Lernmotivation: Untersuchungen zu Entwicklung, Förderung und Wirkung (pp. 183–205). Münster, Germany: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Schlenker, B. R., & Leary, M. R. (1982). Social anxiety and self-presentation: A conceptualization and model. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 641–669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmajuk, N. A., & Buhusi, C. V. (1997). Stimulus configuration, occasion setting, and the hippocampus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 235–257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmalt, H. (1975). Independence training and various aspects of achievement motivation. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 7, 24–37.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1973). Die GITTER-Technik – ein objektives Verfahren zur Messung des Leistungsmotivs bei Kindern [The Grid technique – an objective technique to measure children's achievement motivation]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 5, 231–252.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1976a). Das LM-GITTER. Handanweisung [The AM-Grid. Manual]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1976b). Die Messung des Leistungsmotivs [Measurement of the achievement motive]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1978). Machtmotivation [Power motivation]. Psychologische Rundschau, 30, 269–285.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1979). Leistungsthematische Kognitionen. II: Kausalattribuierungen, Erfolgserwartungen und Affekte [Achievement-related cognitions. II: Causal attributions, expectancies of success, and affect]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 26, 509–531.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1986). Das Machtmotiv und Verantwortlichkeitsattribution für interpersonale Ereignisse [The power motive and attribution of responsibility for interpersonal events]. Psychologische Beiträge, 28, 533–550.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1987). Power motivation and the perception of control. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 101–113). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1996). Zur Kohärenz von Motivation und Kognition [The coherence of motivation and cognition]. In Kuhl, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie. Motivation, Volition und Handeln (pp. 241–273). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (1999). Assessing the achievement motive using the Grid technique. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 109–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D. (2003). Leistungsmotivation im Unterricht: Über den Einsatz des LM-Gitters in der Schule [Achievement motivation in the classroom: Implementing the AM Grid in schools]. In Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. & Rheinberg, F. (Eds.), Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept (pp. 105–127). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H. D., & Langens, T. (1999). Projective, semiprojective and self-report. Measures of human motivation predict private cognitive events: Strivings, memories, and daydreams. Unpublished manuscript, University of Wuppertal, Germany.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D., & Sokolowski, K. (2000). Zum gegenwärtigen Stand der Motivdiagnostik [The current status of motive measurement]. Diagnostica, 46, 115–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D., & Sokolowski, K. (2004). Motivation. In Spada, H. (Ed.), Allgemeine Psychologie (3rd ed.). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Schmalt, H.-D., Sokolowski, K., & Langens, T. (2000). Das Multi-Motiv-Gitter (MMG) [The Multi-Motive Grid (MMG)]. Lisse, Netherlands: Swets.Google Scholar
Schmidt, F. L. (1973). Implications of a measurement problem for expectancy theory research. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 10, 243–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnackers, U., & Kleinbeck, U. (1975). Machtmotiv und machtthematisches Verhalten in einem Verhandlungsspiel [The power motive and power-related behavior in a con game]. Archiv für Psychologie, 127, 300–319.Google Scholar
Schneider, E. L., & Rowe, J. (1990). Handbook of the biology of aging. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schneider, K. (1971). Leistungs- und Risikoverhalten in Abhängigkeit von situativen und überdauernden Komponenten der Leistungsmotivation: Kritische Untersuchungen zu einem Verhaltensmodell [Achievement-related and risk-taking behavior as a function of situational and enduring components of achievement motivation: Critical investigations of a behavioral model]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Schneider, K. (1973). Motivation unter Erfolgsrisiko [Motivation under the risk of success]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schneider, K. (1974). Subjektive Unsicherheit und Aufgabenwahl [Subjective uncertainty and task choice]. Archiv für Psychologie, 126, 147–169.Google Scholar
Schneider, K. (1996). Intrinsisch (autotelisch) motiviertes Verhalten – dargestellt an den Beispielen des Neugierverhaltens sowie verwandter Verhaltenssysteme (Spielen und leistungsmotiviertes Handeln) [Intrinsically (autotelically) motivated behavior – The examples of curiosity and related behavioral systems (playing and achievement-motivated behavior)]. In Kuhl, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Motivation, Volition und Handlung: Enzyklopädie der Psychologie C/IV/4 (pp. 119–153). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., & Dittrich, W. (1990). Evolution und Funktion von Emotionen [Evolution and the function of emotions]. In Scherer, K. R. (Ed.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Psychologie der Emotion (pp. 41–114). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., & Heckhausen, H. (1981). Subjective uncertainty and task preference. In Day, H. I. (Ed.), Advances in intrinsic motivation and aesthetics (pp. 149–167). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, K., & Kreuz, A. (1979). Die Effekte unterschiedlicher Anstrengung auf die Mengen- und Güteleistung bei einer einfachen und schweren Zahlensymbolaufgabe [The effects of effort on the quantity and quality of performance outcomes on easy and difficult number symbol tasks]. Psychologie und Praxis, 23, 34–42.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., & Schmalt, H.-D. (1994). Motivation (2nd ed.). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., & Schmalt, H.-D. (2000). Motivation (3rd ed.). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., & Schmalt, H.-D. (2002). Motivation (4th ed.). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., & Unzner, L. (1992). Preschoolers' attention and emotion in an achievement and an effect game: A longitudindal study. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 37–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, K., Wegge, J., & Konradt, U. (1993). Motivation und Leistung [Motivation and achievement]. In Beckmann, J., Strang, H., & Hahn, E. (Eds.), Aufmerksamkeit und Energetisierung: Facetten von Konzentration und Leistung (pp. 101–131). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schneider, R. J., Hough, L. M., & Dunnette, M. D. (1996). Broadsided by broad traits: How to sink science in five dimensions or less. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 17, 639–655.3.0.CO;2-9>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnierla, J. C. (1959). An evolutionary and developmental theory of biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1959 (pp. 1–42). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Schöne, C., Dickhäuser, O., Spinath, B., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2004). Zielorientierung und Bezugsnormorientierung: zum Zusammenhang zweier Konzepte [Goal orientation and reference-norm orientation: Two related constructs?]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 18, 93–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schore, A. N. (2003). Affect regulation and the repair of self. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). An experimental approach to anxiety, escape, and avoidance behavior. In Hoch, P. M. & Zubin, J. (Eds.), Anxiety (pp. 70–99). New York: Grune & Stratton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schroth, M. L. (1988). Relationships between achievement-related motives, extrinsic conditions, and task performance. Journal of Social Psychology, 127, 39–48.Google Scholar
Schubert, C. (1986). Motivationsanalysen zur Interaktion mit Computern [Motivational analyses of interaction with computers]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Schülein, J. A. (1989). Symbiotische Beziehungen und gesellschaftliche Entwicklung [Symbiotic relations and societal development]. Psyche, 43, 1007–1028.Google Scholar
Schüler, J. (2002). Ein hierarchisches Modell der Anschlussmotivation: Hoffnung-auf-Anschluss und Furcht-vor-Zurückweisung und die Selbstregulation von Zielsetzungen [A hierarchical model of affiliation motivation: Hope for affiliation and fear of rejection and self-regulation of goal setting]. Doctoral dissertation, University of Wuppertal, Germany. Retrieved July 13, 2007, from http://elpub.bib.uni-wuppertal.de/edocs/dokumente/fb03/diss2002/schueler/f030202.pdf.Google Scholar
Schuler, H., & Prochaska, M. (2000). Entwicklung und Konstruktvalidierung eines berufsbezogenen Leistungsmotivationstests [Development and construct validation of a job-related achievement motivation test]. Diagnostica, 46, 61–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C. (in press). Implicit motives. In John, O. P., Robins, R. W. & Pervin, L. A. (Eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. (3 ed.). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C. (2001a). Manual for the assessment of hope of success and fear of failure: English translation of Heckhausen's need achievement measure. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan at Ann Arbor.Google Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C. (2001b). An information processing account of implicit motive arousal. In Maehr, M. L. & Pintrich, P. (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement: Vol. 12. New directions in measures and methods (pp. 1–41). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C. (2007). A biobehavioral model of implicit power motivation: Arousal, reward and frustration. In Harmon-Jones, E. & Winkielman, P. (Eds.), Fundamentals of social neuroscience (pp. 176–196). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Goal imagery: Bridging the gap between implicit motives and explicit goals. Journal of Personality, 67, 1–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2001). Assessment of implicit motives with a research version of the TAT: Picture profiles, gender differences and relations to other personality measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77, 71–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2002). Inhibited power motivation and persuasive communication: A lens model analysis. Journal of Personality, 70, 553–582.Google ScholarPubMed
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2005). An implicit motive perspective on competence. In Elliot, A. J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 31–51). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C., Campbell, K. L., & McClelland, D. C. (1999). Implicit power motivation moderates men's testosterone responses to imagined and real dominance success. Hormones and Behavior, 36, 234–241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultheiss, O. C., Pang, J. S., Torges, C. M., Wirth, M. M., & Treynor, W. (2005). Perceived facial expressions of emotion as motivational incentives: Evidence from a differential implicit learning paradigm. Emotion, 5(1), 41–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultheiss, O. C., & Rohde, W. (2002). Implicit power motivation predicts men's testosteron changes and implicit learning in a contest situation. Hormones and Behavior, 41, 195–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C., Wirth, M. M., Torges, C. M., Pang, J. S., Villacorta, M. A., & Welsh, K. M. (2005). Effects of implicit power motivation on men's and women's implicit learning and testosterone changes after social victory or defeat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(1), 174–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80(1), 1–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275, 1593–1599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, R., & Curnow, C. (1988). Peak performance and age among superathletes: Track and field, swimming, baseball, tennis and golf. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 43, 113–120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (1996). A life-span model of successful aging. American Psychologist, 51, 702–714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, R., Wrosch, C., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). The life-span theory of control: Issues and evidence. In Zarit, S., Pearlin, L., & Schaie, K. W. (Eds.), Personal control in social and life course contexts: Societal impact on aging (pp. 233–262). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Schunk, D. H. (1982). Effects of effort attributional feedback on children's perceived self-efficacy and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 548–556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schur, W., & Weick, G. (1999). Wahnsinnskarriere [Mind-blowing career]. Frankfurt, Germany: Eichhorn.Google Scholar
Schuster, B., Ruble, D. N., & Weinert, F. E. (1998). Causal inferences and the positivity bias in children: The role of the covariation principle. Child Development, 69, 1577–1596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B. (1974). On going back to nature. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 183–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N. (1990). Feeling as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 527–561). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schwarzer, R. (1994). Kausalattributionen als gesundheitsbezogene Kognitionen [Causal attributions as health-related cognitions]. In Försterling, F. & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (Ed.), Attributionstheorie. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1982). Selbstwertdienliche Attributionen nach Leistungsrückmeldungen [Self-serving attri butions after performance feedback]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 14, 47–57.Google Scholar
Gallo, Schweiger I., Achtziger, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2003, September). Vorsatzeffekte bei der Kontrolle von Emotionen [Effects of implementation intentions in the control of emotions]. Paper presented at the Fachtagung Sozialpsychologie, Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Gallo, Schweiger I., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2004, April). Die vorsatzgesteuerte Inhibition der Emotion Ekel [Inhibiting the emotion of disgust by means of implementation intentions]. Paper presented at the symposium “Recent developments in research on implementation intentions” at the 46th meeting of the Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen (TeaP), Giessen, Germany.Google Scholar
Gallo, Schweiger I., Keil, A., Culloch, Mc K. C., Rockstroh, B., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (forthcoming). Strategic Automation of Emotion Control.
Scott, W. A. (1956). The avoidance of threatening material in imaginative behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 338–346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sears, R. R. (1950). Personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 1, 105–118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child rearing.Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.Google Scholar
Sechenow, I. (1863). The reflexes of the brain. Medizinsky Vestnik. In I. Sechenow, Selected works (pp. 263–336). Amsterdam: Bonset, 1968.
Sedikides, C., & Strube, M. J. (1997). Self-evaluation: To thine own self be good, to thine own self be sure, to thine own self be true, and to thine own self be better. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 209–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidenstücker, G., & Seidenstücker, E. (1974). Contribution to a computer evaluation of the Thematic Achievement Motivation Test by Heckhausen. Psychologische Beiträge, 16, 68–92.Google Scholar
Seifert, A. (2001). Fairness leicht gemacht: Zur Rolle von Diskrepanzprozessen in der vorsatzgesteuerten sozialen Urteilsbildung [How to be fair: The role of discrepancy processes and implementation intentions with regard to social judgments]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Konstanz, Germany.Google Scholar
Seligman, M., & Maier, S. F. (1967). Failure to escape traumatic shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(1), 1–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seligman, M. E. P. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review, 77, 406–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2, 307–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P., Abramson, L. Y., Semmel, A., & Baeyer, C. (1979). Depressive attributional style. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 242–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selz, O. (1910). Die experimentelle Untersuchung des Willensaktes [The experimental study of the volitional act]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 57, 241–270.Google Scholar
Selz, O. (1913). Über die Gesetze des geordneten Denkverlaufs [On the laws of ordered thought processes]. Stuttgart, Germany: Spemann.Google Scholar
Semmer, N. (1995). Die Komplexität der Motivation [The complexity of motivation]. Psychoscope, 16(19), 11–15.Google Scholar
Settersten, R. A., & Hagestad, G. (1996). What's the latest? Cultural age deadlines for family transition. Gerontologist, 36, 178–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seward, J. P. (1942). Note on the externalization of drive. Psychological Review, 49, 197–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seward, J. P. (1951). Experimental evidence for the motivating function of reward. Psychological Bulletin, 48, 130–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Forgetting all else: On the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1261–1280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, J. Y., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2000). The structure and substance of intrinsic motivation. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 105–127). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shah, J. Y., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). When opportunity knocks: Bottom-up priming of goals by means and the effects on self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1109–1122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaklee, H. (1983). Causal schemata: Description or explanation of judgment processes? A reply to Fiedler. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1010–1012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Implementation intentions and repeated behavior: Augmenting the predictive validity of the theory of planned behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 349–369.3.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (2000). Using implementation intentions to increase attendance for cervical cancer screening. Health Psychology, 19, 283–289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheeran, P., Webb, T. L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). The interplay between goals and implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 87–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheffield, F. D., & Campbell, B. A. (1954). The role of experience in the “spontaneous” activity of hungry rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 97–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheffield, F. D., & Roby, T. B. (1950). Reward value of non-nutrive sweet taste. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 471–481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheffield, F. D., Roby, T. B., & Campbell, B. A. (1954). Drive reduction versus consummatory behavior as determinants of reinforcement. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 349–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheffield, F. D., Wulff, J. J., & Backer, R. (1951). Reward value of copulation without sex drive reduction. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 44, 3–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 482–497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheldon, K. M., Elliot, A. J., Kim, Y., & Kasser, T. (2001). What is satisfying about satisfying events? Testing 10 candidate psychological needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 325–339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherrington, C. S. (1906). The integrative action of the nervous system.New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Shipley, T. E., & Veroff, J. (1952). A projective measure of need for affiliation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, 349–356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Short, J. -A. C., & Sorrentino, R. M. (1986). Achievement, affiliation, and group incentives: A test of the overmotivation hypothesis. Motivation and Emotion, 10, 115–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Showers, C. J., & Kling, K. C. (1996). Organization of self-knowledge: Implications for recovery from sad mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 578–590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siebert, T., & Vester, T. (1990). Zur Anreizstruktur des Musizierens: Motivationsanalyse einer Tätigkeit [The incentive structure of playing a musical instrument]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Heidelberg, Germany.Google Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (2002). Variability and infant development [Special issue: Variability in infancy]. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 550–557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simmons, R. (1924). The relative effectiveness of certain incentives in animal learning. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 2(Serial no. 7).Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of man.New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Singh, D. (1970). Preference for bar-pressing to obtain reward over freeloading in rats and children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 73, 320–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, S. (1979). Relationships among projective and direct verbal measures of achievement motivation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 43, 45–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1935). Two types of a conditional reflex and a pseudotype. Journal of General Psychology, 12, 66–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental approach.New York: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior.New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1968). The technology of teaching.New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Skinner, E. A. (1990). Age differences in dimensions of perceived control during middle childhood: Implications for developmental conceptualizations and research. Child Development, 61, 1882–1890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 549–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, E. A., Chapman, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1988). Control, means-ends, and agency beliefs: A new conceptualization and its measurement during childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 117–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slavin, R. E. (1995). Cooperative learning (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Smith, A. A. (1953). An electromyographic study of tension in interrupted and completed tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 32–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. P. (Ed.). (1969). The origin and expression of achievement-related motives in children (pp. 102–150). New York: Sage.Google Scholar
Smith, C. P. (Ed.). (1992). Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis.New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. P., & Feld, S. C. (1958). How to learn the method of content analysis for n achievement, n affiliation, and n power. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 685–818). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Smith, D. A. (1971). Lateral hypothalamic stimulation: Experience and deprivation as a factors in rat's licking of empty drinking tubes. Psychological Science, 23, 329–331.Google Scholar
Smith, R. E. (2003). The costs of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: Investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 347–361.Google Scholar
Smock, C. D. (1957). Recall of interrupted and non-interrupted tasks as a function of experimentally induces anxiety and motivational relevance of the task stimuli. Journal of Personality, 25, 589–599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokolov, E. N. (1958). Vospriiate i uslovny refleks.Moscow, Russia: University of Moscow Press.Google Scholar
Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex.New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Sokolowksi, K. (1993). Emotion und Volition [Emotion and volition]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, K. (1986). Kognitionen und Emotionen in anschlussthematischen Situationen [Cognitions and emotions in affiliative situations]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wuppertal, Germany.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, K. (1992). Entwicklung eines Verfahrens zur Messung des Anschlussmotivs [Development of a technique to measure the affiliation motive]. Diagnostica, 38, 1–17.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, K. (1997). Sequentielle und imperative Konzepte des Willens [Sequential and imperative theories of volition]. Psychologische Beiträge, 39, 346–369.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, K. (2002). Emotion. In Prinz, W. & Müsseler, J. (Ed.), Allgemeine Psychologie (pp. 337–384). Heidelberg, Germany: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, K., & Kehr, H. M. (1999). Zum differentiellen Einfluss von Motiven auf die Wirkungen von Führungstrainings (MbO) [Differential impact of motive dispositions on the effects of management trainings (MbO)]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 20, 192–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokolowski, K., & Schmalt, H.-D. (1996). Emotionale und motivationale Einflussfaktoren in einer anschlussthematischen Konfliktsituation [Emotional and motivational influences in an affiliation-related conflict situation]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 18, 461–482.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, K., Schmalt, H.-D., Langens, T., & Puca, R. M. (2000). Assessing achievement, affiliation, and power motives all at once: The Multi-Motive-Grid (MMG). Journal of Personality Assessment, 74, 126–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokolowski, K., Schmitt, S., Jörg, J., & Ringendahl, H. (1997). Anschlussmotiv und Dopamin: Ein Vergleich zwischen Parkinson- und Rheumaerkrankten anhand implizit und explizit gemessener Motive [Implicit versus explicit affiliation motives and dopamine concentration: A comparison of parkinsonian and arthritis patients]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 18, 251– 259.Google Scholar
Solomon, R. L., & Wynne, L. C. (1953). Traumatic avoidance learning: Acquisition in normal dogs. Psychological Monographs, 67(Whole no. 354).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
S⊘rensen, A. B. (1986). Social structure and mechanisms of life-course processes. Human development and the life course: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 177–197). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., Hanna, S. E., & Roney, C. J. R. (1992). Uncertainty orientation. In Smith, C. P. (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 428–439). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., & Hewitt, E. C. (1984). The uncertainty-reducing properties of achievement tasks revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 884–899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., Roney, C. J. E., & Hewitt, E. C. (1988). Information value versus affective value and achievement behavior. In Halisch, F. & Bercken, J. H. L. (Eds.), International perspectives on achievement and task motivation.Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., & Sheppard, B. H. (1978). Effects of affiliation-related motives on swimmers in individual versus group competition: A field experiment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 704–714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., & Short, J.-A. (1977). The case of the mysterious moderates: Why motives sometimes fail to predict behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 478–484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., Short, J. C., & Raynor, J. O. (1984). Uncertainty orientation: Implications for affective and cognitive views of achievement behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 189–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spangler, G., Bräutigam, I., & Stadler, R. (1984). Handlungsentwicklung in der frühen Kindheit und ihre Abhängigkeit von der kognitiven Entwicklung und der emotionalen Erregbarkeit des Kindes [Behavioral development in early childhood and its dependence on the cognitive development and emotional excitability of the child]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 16, 181–193.Google Scholar
Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement: Two meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 140–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spangler, W. D., & House, R. J. (1991). Presidential effectiveness and the leadership motive profile. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 439–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity and femininity.Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1956). Behavior theory and conditioning.New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1960). Behavior theory and learning: Selected papers.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, K. W., Farber, T. E., & McFann, H. H. (1956). The relation of anxiety (drive) level to performance in competitional and noncompetitional paired-associates. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52, 296–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, K. W., & Runquist, W. N. (1958). Temporal effects of conditioned fear on the eyelid reflex. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 613–616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinath, B., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2003). Goal orientation and achievement: The role of ability self-concept and failure perception. Learning and Instruction, 13, 403–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spirito, A., & Hartford, K. (1990). Social skills and depression in adolescent suicide attempters. Adolescence, 25, 543–552.Google ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1996). Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93(24), 13515–13522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A. (1977). Weariness of strangers and the study of infant development. Child Development, 48, 731–746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1979). The coherence of individual development: Early care, attachment and subsequent developmental issues. American Psychologist, 34, 834–841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). The ontogenesis of smiling and laughter: A perspective on the organization of development in infancy. Psychological Review, 83, 173–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stahl, M. J. (1983). Achievement, power, and managerial motivation: Selecting managerial talent with the job choice experience. Personnel Psychology, 36, 775–789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stayton, D. F., Hogan, R., & Ainsworth, M. D. (1971). Infant obedience and maternal behavior: The origins of socialization reconsidered. Child Development, 42(4), 1057–1069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steele, R. S. (1977). Power motivation, activation, and inspirational speeches. Journal of Personality, 45, 53–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steiner, M. (2006). Motivationale Kompetenz und Anreize im Badminton [Motivational competence and intentives in badminton]. Unpublished Lizensiat thesis, University of Zürich, Switzerland.Google Scholar
Steller, B. (1992). Vorsätze und die Wahrnehmung günstiger Gelegenheiten [Intentions and grasping favorable opportunities]. Munich, Germany: tuduv.Google Scholar
Steller, B. (1992). Vorsätze und die Wahrnehmung günstiger Gelegenheiten [Implementation intentions and the perception of viable opportunities to act]. Tuduv: Munich, Germany.Google Scholar
Stern, W. (1935). Allgemeine Psychologie auf personalistischerGrundlage [General psychology: From the personalistic standpoint]. The Hague, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2003). WICS: A model of leadership in organizations. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 2, 386–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, L., & Jones, E. E. (1976). Defensive attribution and the Lelley cube. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 809–820.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, A. J., & Chester, N. L. (1982). Sex differences in human social motives: Achievement, affiliation, and power. In Stewart, A. J. (Ed.), Motivation and society (pp. 172–218). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier, J., Kammer, D., Pelster, A., & Niketta, R. (1985). Attributionsstil und Bewertung als Risikofaktoren der depressiven Reaktion [Attribution style and evaluation as risk factors for depressive symptoms]. Diagnostica, 31, 300–311.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1988). Erlernte Hilflosigkeit, Handlungskontrolle und Leistung [Learned helplessness, action control, and achievement]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1989). Attributional style and depressive mood reactions. Journal of Personality, 57, 581–599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1994). Choice of decision-making strategies and action versus state orientation. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (Eds.), Volition and personality, action versus state orientation (pp. 167–176). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1995). Eine attributionale Analyse elterlichen Erziehungsverhaltens [An attributional analysis of parenting behavior]. In Pawlik, K. (Ed.), Bericht über den 39. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie(Vol. 2, pp. 445–450). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2004). Die Suche nach und das Nachdenken über Ursachen [Searching for and reflecting on causes]. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Giessen, Germany.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Grüner, S. (2005). Reattributionsstraining: Ein Leitfaden [Reattribution training: A guide]. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Giessen, Germany.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Assimi, S. (2002). Attributionale Analyse aggressiven Verhaltens bei Jungen und Mädchen [Attribu tional analysis of aggressive behavior in boys and girls]. In Baumann, M., Keinath, A., & Krems, J. F. (Eds.), Experimentelle Psychologie (p. 65). Regensburg, Germany: Roderer.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., Balke, S., & Schlangen, B. (1996). Lern- versus Leistungszielorientierung als Bedingungen des Lernfortschritts [Learning goal orientation vs. performance goal orientation as determinants of learning progress]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 28, 169–187.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Gerlach, H. (1997). Aggressives Verhalten bei Kindern und Jugendlichen aus attributionstheoretischer Sicht [Aggressive behavior of children and adolescents from an attributional point of view]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 11, 203–209.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Grüner, S. (2005). Reattributionsstraining: Ein Leitfaden [Reattribution training: A guide]. Manuscript submitted for pub lication.
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., Kammer, D., & Adolphs, J. (1988). Attributionsstil und Bewertung bei depressiven versus nichtdepressiven Patienten [Attribution style and evaluation in depressive versus nondepressive patients]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, 17, 46–54.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., Reisenzein, R., & Martini, A. (1995). The role of surprise in the attribution process. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 5–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Rheinberg, F. (Eds.). (2003). Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept [Diagnosis of motivation and self-concept]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., & Schürmann, M. (1991). Attributionsstil als Risikofaktor der depressiven Reaktion bei Kindern [Attribution style as a risk factor for depressive symptoms in children]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 23, 318– 329.Google Scholar
Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., Schürmann, M., Eckert, C., & Pelster, A. (1994). Attributionsstil-Fragebogen für Kinder und Jugendliche (ASF-KJ) [Attribution style questionnaire for children and adolescents (ASF-KJ)]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Stipek, D. (1996). Motivation and instruction. In Berliner, D. C. & Calfee, R. C. (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 85–113). New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Stipek, D., & Daniels, D. (1990). Children's use of dispositional attributions in predicting the performance and behavior of classmates. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 11, 13–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stipek, D., & Gralinski, J. H. (1996). Children's beliefs about intelligence and school performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 397–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stipek, D., & Hoffman, J. (1980). Development of children's performance-related judgements. Child Development, 51, 912–914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stipek, D., & Kowalski, P. (1989). Learned helplessness in task-orienting versus performance-orienting testing conditions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3), 384–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stipek, D., Recchia, S., & McClintic, S. (1992). Self-evaluation in young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57(1).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stipek, D., & Tannatt, L. (1984). Children's judgments of their own and peers' academic competence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 75–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stipek, D. J., & Decotis, K. (1988). Children's understanding of the implications of causal attributions for emotional experiences. Child Development, 59, 1601–1616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, P. J., Dumphy, D. C., Smith, M. S., & Ogilvie, D. M. (1966). The general inquirer.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stricker, E. M., & Verbalis, J. G. (2002). Hormones and ingestive behaviors. In Becker, J. B., Breedlove, S. M., & Crews, D. (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinology (2nd ed., pp. 451–473). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stroebe, W., Eagly, A. H., & Stroebe, M. S. (1977). Friendly or just polite? The effect of self-esteem on attributions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 7, 265– 274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stuchlikova, I., & Man, F. (1999). Motivational structure of state and action oriented alcoholics. Studia Psychologica, 41, 63–72.Google Scholar
Stumpf, H., Angleitner, A., Wieck, T., Jackson, D. N., & Beloch-Till, H. (1985). Deutsche Personality Research Form (PRF) [German Personality Research Form (PRF)]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Stutz, A. M., Morrison, C. D., & Argyropoulos, G. (2005). The agouti-related protein and its role in energy homeostasis. Peptides, 26(10), 1771–1781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sueton Tranquilinas. De vita Caesarum.
Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry.New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Sullivan, R. M., Wilson, D. A., Wong, R., Correa, A., & Leon, M. (1990). Modified behavioral and olfactory bulb responses to maternal odors in preweanling rats. Brain Research. Developmental Brain Research, 53(2), 243–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surber, C. F. (1980). The development of reversible operations in judgments of ability, effort and performance. Child Development, 51, 1018–1029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutherland, R. W., & Rudy, J. W. (1989). Configurational association theory: The role of hippocampal formation in learning, memory and amnesia. Psychobiology, 17, 129–144.Google Scholar
Sutton, S. K., & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Prefrontal brain asymmetry: A biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems. Psychological Science, 8, 204–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svenson, G. R., Oestergren, P.-O., Merlo, J., & Rastam, L. (2002). Action control and situational risks in the prevention of risks HIV and STIs: Individual, dyadic, and social influences on consistent condom use in a university population. AIDS Education and Prevention, 14, 515–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swithers, S. E., & Martinson, F. A. (1998). Habituation of oral responding in adult rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112(1), 213–224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taira, K., & Rolls, E. T. (1996). Receiving grooming as a reinforcer for the monkey. Physiology & Behavior, 59(6), 1189–1192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taub, E., & Berman, A. J. (1968). Movement and learning in the absence of sensory feedback. In Freedman, S. J. (Ed.), The neuropsychology of spatially oriented behavior (pp. 173–192). Homewood, IL: Dorsey.Google Scholar
Tauer, J. M., & Harackiewicz, (1999). Winning isn't everything: Competition, achievement orientation, intrinsic motivation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 209–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, J. A. (1953). A personality scale of manifest anxiety. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48, 285–290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, J. A., & Spence, K. W. (1952). The relationship of anxiety level to performance in serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 61–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social-psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. (1994). Positive illusions and well-being revisited: Separating fact from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 21–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. E., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1995). Effects of mindset on positive illusions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 213–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. E., Neter, E., & Wayment, H. A. (1995). Self-evaluation processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1278–1287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teachman, J. D., & Heckhert, A. (1985). The impact of age and children on remarriage. Journal of Family Issues, 6, 185–203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teevan, R. C., & McGhee, P. E. (1972). Childhood development of the fear of failure motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 345–348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terhune, K. W. (1968a). Motives, situation, and interpersonal conflict within Prisoner's Dilemma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Monograph Supplement 8, 1–24.Google Scholar
Terhune, K. W. (1968b). Studies of motives, cooperation, and conflict within laboratory microcosms. Buffalo Studies, 4 (1), 29–58.Google Scholar
The Oxford English Dictionary (2007). Volume IX. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Thierry, D. (2004). Financial compensation at work: A motivational mess? In Wegge, J. & Schmidt, K.-H. (Eds.), Förderung von Arbeitsmotivation und Gesundheit in Organisationen. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Thomae, H. (Ed.). (1965). Handbuch der Psychologie. Allgemeine Psychologie II: Motivation [Handbook of psychology. Germany psychology II: Motivation]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Thomas, E. A. C. (1983). Notes on effort and achievement oriented behavior. Psychological Review, 90, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R. (1987). Development of children's inferences of the emotions of others. Developmental Psychology, 22, 124–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R., Gupta, S., Miller, K., Mills, S., & Orr, S. (2004). The effects of vasopressin on human facial responses related to social communication. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 35–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of associative processes in animals. Psychological Review, Monographs Supplement 5, 551–553.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1913). Educational psychology. New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Implicit and self-attributed achievement motives: Concordance and predictive validity. Journal of Personality, 70, 729–755.Google ScholarPubMed
Thurstone, L. L. (1937). Ability, motivation, and speed. Psychometrika, 2, 249–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiedens, L. Z., & Fragale, A. R. (2003). Complementarity in dominant and submissive nonverbal behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 558–568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct.London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Toates, F. M. (1981). The control of ingestive behavior by internal and external stimuli: A theoretical review. Appetite, 2, 35–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toates, F. M. (1986). Motivational systems. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1926). The nature of fundamental drives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 20, 349–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men.New York: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1951). A psychological model. In Parsons, T. & Shils, E. (Eds.), Toward a general theory of action (pp. 279–361). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1952). A cognition motivation model. Psychological Review, 59, 389–400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolman, E. C. (1959). Principles of purposive behavior. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. II, pp. 92–157). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Toman, W. (1960). On the periodicity of motivation. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1960 (pp. 80–96). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Tomasik, M. J. (2003). Adjusting goal aspirations when getting close to a developmental deadline: The role of primary and secondary control strategies. Unpublished Diplom thesis, Free University of Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, and consciousness. Vol. 1. The positive affects. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S. (1970). Affect as the primary motivational system. In Arnold, M. (Ed.), Feelings and emotions (pp.101–111). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S. (1981). The quest for primary motives: Biography and autobiography of an idea. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 306–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1977). Descriptive analyses of infant communicative behavior. In Schaffer, H. R. (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp. 227–270). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1980). The foundations of intersubjectivity: Development of interpersonal and cooperative understanding in infants. The social foundations of language and thought: Essays in honor of J. S. Bruner. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: McLeod.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 3–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding, and acts of meaning in the first year. Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 183–229). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1972). The analysis of subjective culture. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1997). Cross-cultural perspectives on personality. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 440–464). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Triemer, A. (2001). Ambulantes psychophysiologisches 24-Stunden-Monitoring zur Erfassung von arbeitsbezogenen Stimmungen und Emotionen [Ambulant psychophysiological 24-hour monitoring to assess work-related mood and emotion]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Technical University of Dresden, Germany.Google Scholar
Triemer, A., & Rau, R. (2001). Stimmungskurven im Arbeitsalltag – eine Feldstudie [Mood curves at normal working days – A field study]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 22, 42–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. (1975). Seeking information about one's own ability as a determinant of choice among tasks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 1004–1013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. (1980). Self-assessment, self-enhancement, and task preference. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 116–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. (1983). Self-assessment in achievement behavior. In Suls, J. M. & Greenwald, A. G. (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 2, pp. 93–121). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Trope, Y. (1986a). Identification and inferential processes in dispositional attribution. Psychological Review, 93, 239–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. (1986b). Testing self-enhancement and self-enhancement theories of achievement motivation: A reply to Sohn's critique. Motivation and Emotion, 10, 247–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. (1986c). Self-enhancement and self-assessment in achievement behavior. In Sorrentino, R. M. & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 350–378). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Trope, Y., & Brickman, P. (1975). Difficulty and diagnosticity as determinants of choice among tasks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 918–926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trötschel, R., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2004). Verhandlungsführung – Psychologische Grundlagen [Negotiation – Psychological fundaments]. In Sommer, G. & Fuchs, A. (Eds.), Lehrbuch Konflikt- und Friedenspsychologie (pp. 158–177). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz, PVU.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C. (1975). Häusliche Umwelt und Motiventwicklung [Home environment and motivation]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C. (1982a). The development of achievement motivation and individual differences: Ecological determinants. In Hartup, W. W. (Ed.), Review of child development research (Vol. 6, pp. 669–703). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C. (1982b). Der ökologische Ansatz in der Erforschung der Leistungsmotivgenese [The ecological approach in research on the development of the achievement motive]. In Vaskowics, L. (Ed.), Umweltbedingungen familialer Sozialisation (pp. 168–203). Stuttgart, Germany: Enke.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C. (1987). The role of toys and games in an ecological approach to motive development. In Halisch, F. & Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 179–199). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trudewind, C. (2000). Curiosity and anxiety as motivational determinants of cognitive development. In Heckhausen, J. (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 15–38). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C., Brünger, T., & Krieger, K. (1986). Parental expectations and the development of achievement motivation. In Bercken, J. H., DeBruyn, E., & Bergen, T. (Eds.), Achievement and task motivation (pp. 179–200). Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C., & Husarek, B. (1979). Mutter-Kind-Interaktion bei der Hausaufgabenbetreuung und die Leistungsmotiventwicklung im Grundschulalter: Analyse einer ökologischen Schlüsselsituation [Mother-child interaction in homework supervision and the development of the achievement motive in primary school age: Analysis of a key ecological situation]. In Walter, H. & Oerter, R. (Eds.), Ökologie und Entwicklung (pp. 229–246). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C., & Schneider, K. (1994). Individual differences in the development of exploratory behavior: Methodological considerations. In Keller, H., Schneider, K., & Henderson, B. (Eds.), Curiosity and exploration (pp. 151–176). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trudewind, C., Unzner, L., & Schneider, K. (1997). Die Entwicklung der Leistungsmotivation [The development of achievement motivation]. In Keller, H. (Ed.), Handbuch der Kleinkindforschung (pp. 587–622). Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Trudewind, C., & Windel, A. (1991). Elterliche Einflussnahme auf die kindliche Kompetenzentwicklung: Schulleistungsentwicklung und ihre motivationale Vermittlung [Parental influence on the development of child competence: Development of school achievement and its motivational antecedents]. In Pekrun, R. & Fend, H. (Eds.), Schule und Persönlichkeitsentwicklung (pp. 131–148). Stuttgart, Germany: Enke.Google Scholar
Tucker, D. M., & Williamson, P. A. (1984). Asymmetric neural control systems in human self-regulation. Psychological Re view, 91, 185–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuerlinckx, F., Boeck, P., & Lens, W. (2002). Measuring needs with the Thematic Apperception Test: A psychometric study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 448–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there?American Psychologist, 40, 495–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tupes, E. C., & Christal, R. C. (1992). Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings. Journal of Personality, 60, 225–252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tweer, R. (1976). Das Ökonomieprinzip in der Anstrengungskalkulation: Eine entwicklungspsychologische Untersuchung [The parsimony principle in the calculation of effort: An investigation from the perspective of developmental psychology]. Unpublished Diplom thesis, University of Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2001). Age and birth cohort differences in self-esteem: A cross temporal meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 321–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Schonen, S., Crivello, F., Reutter, B., Aujard, Y., & Mazoyer, B. (2002). Neural correlates of woman face processing by 2-month-old infants. NeuroImage, 15, 454–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uhlenberg, P. (1974). Cohort variations in family life cycle experiences of U.S. females. Journal of Marriage and Family, 36, 284–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uleman, J. S. (1966). A new TAT measure of the need for power. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Uleman, J. S. (1971). Dyadic influence in an “ESP study” and TAT measures of the needs for influence and power. Journal of Personality Assessment, 35, 248–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uleman, J. S. (1972). The need for influence: Development and validation of a measure, and comparison with the need for power. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 85, 157–214.Google Scholar
Undeutsch, U., & Hermans, H. (1976). Leistungsmotivationstest für Jugendliche (LMT-J) [Achievement motivation test for adolescents]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Ungless, M. A. (2004). Dopamine: the salient issue. Trends in Neuroscience, 27, 702–706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uvnaes-Moberg, K. (1998). Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 819–835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentine, C. W. (1930). The innate bases of fear. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 37, 394–419.Google Scholar
Lawick-Goodall, J. (1968). The behavior of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Area. Animal Behavior Monographs, 1, 161–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1975). The behavior of the chimpanzee. In Kurth, G. & Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (Eds.), Hominisation und Verhalten. Stuttgart, Germany: Fischer.Google Scholar
Overwalle, F., & Heylighen, F. (1995). Relating covariation information to causal dimensions through principles of contrast and invariance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 435–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veroff, J. (1957). Development and validation of a projective measure of power motivation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veroff, J. (1969). Social comparison and the development of achievement motivation. In Smith, C. P. (Ed.), Achievement-related motives in children (pp. 46–101). New York: Sage.Google Scholar
Veroff, J. (1982). Assertive motivations: Achievement versus power. In Winter, D. G. & Stewart, A. J. (Eds.), Motivation and society. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Veroff, J. (1992). Power motivation. In Smith, C. P. (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 278–285). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veroff, J., Atkinson, J. W., Feld, S. C., & Gurin, G. (1960). The use of thematic apperception to assess motivation in a nationwide interview study. Psychological Monographs, 74 (12, Whole no. 499).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veroff, J., Depner, C., Kulka, R., & Douvan, E. (1980). Comparison of American motives: 1957 versus 1976. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1249–1262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veroff, J., & Feld, S. C. (1970). Marriage and work in America. New York: Van Nostrand-Reinhold.Google Scholar
Veroff, J., Reuman, D., & Feld, S. C. (1984). Motives in American men and women across the adult life span. Developmental Psychology, 20, 1142–1158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veroff, J., & Veroff, J. B. (1972). Reconsideration of a measure of power motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 78, 279–291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volling, B. L., McElwain, N. L., Notaro, P. C., & Herrera, C. U. (2002). Parents' emotional availability and infant emotional competence: Predictors of parent-infant attachment and emerging self-regulation. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 447–465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vollmeyer, R., & Rheinberg, F. (2003, August). Task difficulty and flow. Paper presented at the biennial EARLI conference, Padova, Italy.Google Scholar
Vontobel, J. (1970). Leistungsbedürfnis und soziale Umwelt [Need for achievement and the social environment]. Berne, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation.New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Vuilleumier, P., Richardson, M. P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7 (11), 1271–1278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vygotski, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1957). The strategy of the genes.London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Wadsworth, M., &. Ford, D. (1983). Assessment of personal goal hierarchies. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 30, 514–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. (1969). Levels of symbolization in adolescent adjustment patterns. International Journal of Symbology, 1 (1), 67–74.Google Scholar
Wahba, M. A., & House, R. J. (1974). Expectancy theory in work and motivation: Some logical and methodological issues. Human Relations, 27, 121–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wainer, H. A., & Rubin, I. M. (1971). Motivation of research and development entrepreneurs: Determinants of company success. In Kolb, D. A., Rubin, I. M., & McIntire, J. (Eds.), Organizational psychology (pp. 131–139). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Walker, E. L. (1969). Reinforcement – The one ring. In Trapp, J. T. (Ed.), Reinforcement and behavior (pp. 47–62). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Walker, E. L. (1973). Psychological complexity and preference: A hedgehog theory of behavior. In Berlyne, D. E. & Madsen, K. B. (Eds.), Pleasure, reward, preference (pp. 65–97). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Walker, E. L., & Heyns, R. W. (1962). Conformity and conflict of needs. In Walter, E. L. & Heyns, R. W. (Eds.), Anatomy for conformity (pp. 54–68). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Wallen, K. (2001). Sex and context: Hormones and primate sexual motivation. Hormones and Behavior, 40 (2), 339–357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walschburger, P. (1994). Action control and excessive demand: Effects of situational and personality factors on psychological and physiological functions during stressful transactions. In Kuhl, J. & Beckmann, J. (eds.), Volition and personality (pp. 233–266). Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.Google Scholar
Walster, E. (1964). The temporal sequence of post-decision processes. In Festinger, L. (Ed.), Conflict, decision, and dissonance (pp. 112–127). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Warden, C. J., Jenkins, T. N., & Warner, L. H. (1936). Comparative psychology. New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
Wasna, M. (1970). Die Entwicklung der Leistungsmotivation [The development of achievement motivation]. Munich, Germany: Reinhardt.Google Scholar
Watson, D. (1982). The actor and the observer: How are their perceptions of causality divergent?Psychological Bulletin, 92, 682–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Extraversion and its positive emotional core. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 767–793). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 820–838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1919). Psychology from the standpoint of a behaviorist.Philadelphia: Lippincott.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1924). Behaviorism.New York: People's Institute Company.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B., & Morgan, J. J. B. (1917). Emotional reactions and psychological experimentation. American Journal of Psychology, 28, 163–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. S. (1966). The development and generalization of contingency awareness in early infancy: Some hypotheses. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 12, 123–135.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S. (1972). Smiling, cooing, and “The Game.”Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 323–339.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S., & Ramey, C. (1972). Reactions to response contingent stimulation in early infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 219–228.Google Scholar
Watt, H. J. (1905). Experimentelle Beiträge zu einer Theorie des Denkens [Experimental contributions to a theory of thought]. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 4, 289–436.Google Scholar
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Can implementation intentions help to overcome ego-depletion?Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 279–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber, M. (1904). Die protestantische Ethik und der Geist des Kapitalismus [The Protestant ethic and the spirit of capitalism]. Archiv für Sozialwissenschaft und Sozialpolitik, 20, 1–54.Google Scholar
Weber, M. (1921). III. Abteilung. Wirtschaft und Gesellschaft. 1. Die Wirtschaft und die gesellschaftlichen Ordnungen und Mächte. Grundriss der Sozialökonomik [Economy and society. 1. The economy and social powers and structures] (New ed. 1964). Tübingen, Germany: Mohr-Siebeck.Google Scholar
Weber, M. (1947). The theory of social and economic organization. The theory of social and economic organization [1st American Edition (A. M. Henderson & T. Parsons, Trans.)]. Oxford University Press: New York, NY.Google Scholar
Wegge, J., Quaeck, A., & Kleinbeck, U. (1996). Zur Faszinationskraft von Video- und Computerspielen bei Studenten: Welche Motive befriedigen die “bunte Welt am Draht”? [The fascination of video and computer games among students]. In Bräuer, K. & Kittler, U. (Eds.), Pädagogische Psychologie und ihre Anwendungen (Vol. 2, pp. 51–76). Essen, Germany: Die blaue Eule.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 35–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M., & Wheatley, T. (1999). Apparent mental causation: Sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist, 54, 480–492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberger, J., & McClelland, D. C. (1990). Cognitive versus traditional motivational models: Irreconcilable or complementary? In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 562–597). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Weinberger, J., & Silverman, L. H. (1987). Subliminal psychodynamic activation: A method for studying psychoanalytic dynamic propositions. In Hogan, R. & Jones, H. (Eds.), Perspective in personality (Vol. 2, pp. 251–287). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1965a). Need achievement and the resumption of incompleted tasks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 165–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1965b). The effects of unsatisfied achievement motivation on persistence and subsequent performance. Journal of Personality, 33, 428–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1966). Role of success and failure in the learning of easy and complex tasks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 339–344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, B. (1967). Implications of the current theory of achievement motivation for research and performance in the classroom. Psychology in the School, 4, 164–171.3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1970). New conceptions in the study of achievement motivation. In Maher, B. (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality research (Vol. 5, pp. 67–109). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1972). Theories of motivation.Chicago: Markham.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1974). Achievement motivation and attribution theory.Morristown, NJ: General Learning.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 3–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, B. (1980). A cognitive (attribution) – emotion – action model of motivated behavior: An analysis of judgments of help-giving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 186–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1985a). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548–573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1985b). “Spontaneous” causal thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 74–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion.New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1992). Human motivation. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1994). Sünde versus Krankheit [Sin versus illness]. In Försterling, F. & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (Eds.), Attributionspsychologie (pp. 1–25). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility. A foundation for a theory of social conduct. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., Frieze, I. H., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., & Rosenbaum, R. M. (1971). Perceiving the causes of success and failure.New York: General Learning.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., Heckhausen, H., Meyer, W.-U., & Cook, R. E. (1972). Causal ascriptions and achievement behavior: A conceptual analysis of effort and reanalysis of locus of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 239–248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, B., & Kukla, A. (1970). An attributional analysis of achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B., Kun, A., & Benesh-Weiner, M. (1980). The development of mastery, emotions, and morality from an attributional perspective. Minnesota Symposia on Child Development, 13, 103–129.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., & Peter, N. (1973). A cognitive-developmental analysis of achievement and moral judgments. Developmental Psychology, 9, 290–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B., & Schneider, K. (1971). Drive versus cognitive theory: A reply to Boor and Harmon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 258–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinert, A. B. (2001). Berufsorientierungstest [Career orientation test]. In Wottawa, H. & Sarges, W. (Eds.), Handbuch Wirtschaftspsychologischer Testverfahren (pp. 125–130). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst.Google Scholar
Weinert, F. E. (1991). Vorwort zur deutschsprachigen Ausgabe [Preface to the German edition]. In Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.), Die außergewöhnliche Erfahrung im Alltag: Die Psychologie des Flow-Erlebens (pp. 7–9). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Weisfeld, G. E., & Beresford, J. M. (1982). Erectedness of posture as an indicator of dominance or success in humans. Motivation and Emotion, 6, 113–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, A., King, J. E., & Enns, R. M. (2002). Subjective well-being is heritable and genetically correlated with dominance in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1141–1149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, A., King, J. E., & Figueredo, A. J. (2000). The heritability of personality factors in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavior Genetics, 30, 213–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisz, J. R. (1983). Can I control it? The pursuit of veridical answers across the life span. In Baltes, P. B. & Brim, O. G. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 3) (pp. 233–300). Orlando, Forida: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Weisz, J. R., Yeates, K. O., Robertson, D., & Beckmann, J. C. (1982). Perceived contingency of skill and chance events: A developmental analysis. Developmental Psychology, 18, 898–905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welker, W. L. (1956). Some determinants of play and exploration in chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 49, 84–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, G. L., & Harvey, J. H. (1977). Do people use consensus information in making causal attributions?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 279–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wendt, H. W. (1955). Motivation, effort, and performance. In McClelland, D. C. (Ed.), Studies in motivation (pp. 448–459). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Wendt, H. W. (1967). Verhaltensmodelle des Nichtwissenschaftlers: Einige biographische und Antriebskorrelate der wahrgenommenen Beziehung zwischen Erfolgswahrscheinlichkeit und Zielanreiz [Models of a nonscientist's behavior: Some biographical and motivational correlates of the perceived relationship between the probability of success and incentive value]. Psychologische Forschung, 30, 226–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wentzel, K. R. (1989). Adolescent classroom goals, standards for performance and academic achievement: An interactionist perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81 (2), 131–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wertlieb, D., Weigel, C., & Feldstein, M. (1987). Measuring children's coping. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57 (4), 548–560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, M. M., & Konner, M. J. (1976). The role of the father: An anthropological perspective.New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Westergaard, G. C., Suomi, S. J., Higley, J. D., & Mehlman, P. T. (1999). Csf 5-hiaa and aggression in female macaque monkeys: Species and interindividual differences. Psychopharmacology, 146 (4), 440–446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 331–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, G. M. (1980). Conceptual universals in interpersonal language. American Anthropologist, 82, 759–781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, R. W. (1960). Competence and the psychosexual stages of development. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1960 (pp. 97–140). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Wicklund, R. A., & Brehm, J. W. (1976). Perspectives on cognitive dissonance. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wicklund, R. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion.Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wicklund, R. A. (1986). Orientation to the environment versus preoccupation with human potential. In Sorrentino, R. M., & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 64–95). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wiese, B. S. (2000). Berufliche und familiäre Zielstrukturen [Occupational and family goal structures]. Münster, Germany: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Wiese, B. S., & Freund, A. M. (2000). The interplay of work and family in young and middle adulthood. In Heckhausen, J. (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 233–249). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Wiese, R. A., & Rompré, P. P. (1989). Brain dopamine and reward. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 191–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1997). Changes in children's competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elementary school years. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 451–469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S, Yoon, K. S., & Harold, R. D. (1997). Change in children's competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elementary school years: A 3-year study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 451–469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, D. R., & Williams, H. (1969). Auto-maintenance in the pigeon: Sustained pecking despite contingent non-reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 511–520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, G. C. (1957). Pleitropy, natural selection, and the evolution of senescence. Evolution, 11, 398–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, S. B. (1938). Resistance to extinction as a function of the number of reinforcements. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 506–521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1980). Sociobiology: The abridged edition. Cambridge, MA: Belknap(Harvard.Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive unconscious.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wine, J. (1971). Test anxiety and direction of attention. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 92–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wine, J. (1982). Evaluation anxiety: A cognitive-attentional construct. In Krohne, H. W. & Laux, L. C. (Eds.), Achievement, stress, and anxiety (pp. 207–219). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Wingfield, J. C., Hegner, R. E., Dufty, A. M., & Ball, G. F. (1990). The “challenge hypothesis”: Theoretical implications for patterns of testosterone secretion, mating systems, and breeding strategies. The American Naturalist, 136, 829–846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winslow, J. T., & Insel, T. R. (2002). The social deficits of the oxytocin knockout mouse. Neuropeptides, 36 (2–3), 221–229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, D. G. (1967). Power motivation in thought and action. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1973). The power motive. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1987a). Enhancement of an enemy's power motivation as a dynamic of conflict escalation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 41–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1987b). Leader appeal, leader performance, and the motive profiles of leaders and followers: A study of American presidents and elections. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 196–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1988). The power motive in women – and men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 510–519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1991a). Manual for scoring motive imagery in running text (3rd ed.). Unpublished scoring manual, University of Michigan at Ann Arbor.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1991b). Measuring personality at a distance: Development of an integrated system for scoring motives in running text. In Hogan, R. (Series Ed.) & Ozer, D., Healy, J. M., & Stewart, A. J. (Vol. Eds.), Perspectives in personality: Vol. 3B. Approaches to understanding lives (pp. 59–89). London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1993). Power, affiliation, and war: Three tests of a motivational model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 532–545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1996). Personality: Analysis and interpretation of lives.New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (2001). Measuring Bush's motives. Proceedings of the International Society for Political Psychology. ISPP News, 8–9.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G., & Barenbaum, N. B. (1985). Responsibility and the power motive in women and men. Journal of Personality, 53, 335–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D. G., John, O. P., Stewart, A. J., Klohnen, E. C., & Duncan, L. E. (1998). Traits and motives: Toward an integration of two traditions in personality research. Psychological Review, 105, 230–250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, D. G., & Stewart, A. J. (1977). Power motive reliability as a function of retest instructions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 436–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D. G., & Stewart, A. J. (1978). Power motivation. In London, H. & Exner, J. (Eds.), Dimensions of personality (pp. 391–447). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G., Stewart, A., John, O. P., Klohnen, E. C., & Duncan, L. E. (1998). Traits and motives: Toward an integration of two traditions in personality research. Psychological Review, 105, 230–250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, D. G., & Wiecking, F. A. (1971). The new Puritans: Achievement and power motives of New Left radicals. Behavioral Science, 16, 523–530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winterbottom, M. R. (1953). The relation of childhood training in independence to achievement motivation. Dissertation Abstracts, 13, 440–441.Google Scholar
Winterbottom, M. R. (1958). The relation of need for achievement to learning experiences in independence and mastery. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action and society (pp. 453–478). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Wirth, M. M., Welsh, K. M., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2006). Salivary cortisol changes in humans after winning or losing a dominance contest depend on implicit power motivation. Hormones and Behavior, 49 (3), 346–352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wise, R. A., & Rompré, P.-P. (1989). Brain dopamine and reward. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 191–225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witkowski, T., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1998). Performance deficits following failure: Learned helplessness or self-esteem protection?British Journal of Educational Psychology, 37, 59–71.Google ScholarPubMed
Wittling, W. (1990). Psychophysiological correlates of human brain asymmetry: Blood pressure changes during lateralized presentation of an emotionally laden film. Neuropsychologia, 28, 457–470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woike, B. A. (1995). Most memorable experiences: Evidence for a link between implicit and explicit motives and social cognitive processes in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1081–1091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woike, B., Gershkowich, I., Piorkowski, R., & Poco, M. (1999). The role of motives in the content and structure of autobiographical memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 600–612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woike, B., & Poco, M. (2001). Motive-related memories: Content, structure, and affect. Journal of Personality, 69, 391–415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolters, C. A. (2003). Regulation of motivation: Evaluating an underemphasized aspect of self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 189–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, D. J., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, R. E. (1986). Task complexity: Definition of the construct. Organizational Behavior and Decision Processes, 37, 60–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, R. E., & Locke, E. A. (1990). Goal setting and strategy effects on complex tasks. In Staw, B. & Cummings, L. (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 73–110). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Wood, R. E., Mento, A. J., & Locke, E. A. (1987). Task complexity as a moderator of goal effects: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 416–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodson, J. C. (2002). Including “learned sexuality” in the organization of sexual behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 69–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. (1918). Dynamic psychology.New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Worchel, P. (1957). Adaptability screening of flying personnel: Development of a self-concept inventory for predicting maladjustment. ASAF School of Aviation Medicine Report, No. 56–62.
Wright, R. (1994). The moral animal: Evolutionary psychology and everyday life.New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Wright, R. A. (1996). Brehm's theory of motivation as a model of effort and cardiovascular response. In Gollwitzer, P. M. & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 424–453). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wrosch, C., & Freund, A. (2001). Self-regulation of normative and non-normative developmental challenges. Human Development, 44 (5), 264–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrosch, C., & Heckhausen, J. (1999). Control processes before and after passing a developmental deadline: Activation and reactivation of intimate relationship goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 415–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrosch, C., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). Perceived control of life regrets: Good for young and bad for old adults. Psychology and Aging, 17, 340–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrosch, C., Heckhausen, J., & Lachman, M. E. (2000). Primary and secondary control strategies for managing health and financial stress across adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 15, 387–399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrosch, C., Miller, G. E., Scheier, M. F., & Pontet, C. (2007). Giving up on unattainable goals: Benefits for health?Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 251–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., Miller, G. E., Schulz, R., & Carver, C. S. (2003). Adaptive self-regulation of unattainable goals: Goal disengagement, goal reengagement and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29 (12), 1494–1508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrosch, C., Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). Health stresses and depressive symptomatology in the elderly: The importance of health engagement control strategies. Health Psychology, 21 (4), 340–348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrosch, C., Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (2004). Health stresses and depressive symptomatology in the elderly: A control-process approach. Current Directions, 13, 17–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wundt, W. (1874). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie [Principles of physiological psychology]. Leipzig, Germany: Engelmann.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1894). Über psychische Causalität und das Princip des psychophysischen Parallelismus [On psychic causality and the principle of psychopsychic parallelism]. Philosophische Studien, 10, 1–124.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1896). Grundriß der Psychologie [Outlines of psychology]. Leipzig, Germany: Engelmann.Google Scholar
Wynne-Edwards, K. E. (2001). Hormonal changes in mammalian fathers. Hormones and Behavior, 40, 139–145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamaguchi, S., & Ninomiya, K. (2000). Umami and food palatability. Journal of Nutrition, 130 (4 Suppl), 921–926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yarrow, L. J., McQuiston, S., MacTurk, R. H., McCarthy, M. E., Klein, R. P., & Vietze, P. M. (1983). Assessment of mastery motivation during the first year of life: Contemporaneous and cross-age relationships. Developmental Psychology, 19, 159–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative and Neurological Psychology, 18, 459–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yerkes, R. M., & Morgulis, S. (1909). The method of Pavlov in animal psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 6, 257–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, L. J., & Insel, T. R. (2002). Hormones and parental behavior. In Becker, J. B., Breedlove, S. M., Crews, D., & McCarthy, M. M. (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinology (2nd ed., pp. 331–369). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Young, P. T. (1936). Motivation of behavior: The fundamental determinants of human and animal activity. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, P. T. (1941). The experimental analysis of appetite. Psychological Bulletin, 38, 129–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, P. T. (1949). Food-seeking drive, affective process, and learning. Psychological Review, 56, 98–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, P. T. (1959). The role of affective processes in learning and motivation. Psychological Review, 66, 104–125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, P. T. (1961). Motivation and emotion: A survey of the determinants of human and animal activity. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yussen, S., & Kane, P. (1985). Children's conceptions of intelligence. In Yussen, S. R. (Ed.), The growth of reflection in children (pp. 207–241). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Cognitive theories in social psychology. In Lindzey, G. & Aronson, E. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. I, 2nd ed., pp. 320–411). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zak, P. J., Kurzban, R., & Matzner, W. T. (2005). Oxytocin is associated with human trustworthiness. Hormones and Behavior, 48 (5), 522–527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeaman, D. (1949). Response latency as a function of the amount of reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 466–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zehr, J. L., Maestripieri, D., & Wallen, K. (1998). Estradiol increases female sexual initiation independent of male responsiveness in rhesus monkeys. Hormones and Behavior, 33, 95–103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Über das Behalten von erledigten und unerledigten Handlungen [The retention of completed and uncompleted activities]. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1–85.Google Scholar
Zepelin, H., Sills, R. A., & Heath, M. W. (1986–87). Is age becoming irrelevant? An exploratory study of perceived age norms. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 241–256.Google Scholar
Ziegler, A., & Heller, K. A. (2000). Effects of an attribution retraining with female students gifted in physics. Journal of the Education of the Gifted, 23, 217–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, A., & Stöger, H. (2004). Evaluation of an attributional retraining (modeling technique) to reduce gender differences in chemistry instruction. High Ability Studies, 15, 63–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimbardo, P. (ed.) (1969). The cognitive control of motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (1997). Developmental phases in self-regulation: Shifting from process goals to outcome goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 29–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, M., & Lubin, B. (1965). Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist: Today form. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.Google Scholar
Zumkley, H. (1978). Aggression und Katharsis [Aggression and catharsis]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Zurbriggen, E. L. (2000). Social motives and cognitive power-sex associations: Predictors of aggressive sexual behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 559–581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Edited by Jutta Heckhausen, University of California, Irvine, Heinz Heckhausen, Max-Planck-Institut für Psychologische Forschung, Munich
  • Book: Motivation and Action
  • Online publication: 08 August 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511499821.017
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Edited by Jutta Heckhausen, University of California, Irvine, Heinz Heckhausen, Max-Planck-Institut für Psychologische Forschung, Munich
  • Book: Motivation and Action
  • Online publication: 08 August 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511499821.017
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Edited by Jutta Heckhausen, University of California, Irvine, Heinz Heckhausen, Max-Planck-Institut für Psychologische Forschung, Munich
  • Book: Motivation and Action
  • Online publication: 08 August 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511499821.017
Available formats
×